Category: Diet

Fatigue and exercise performance

Fatigue and exercise performance

Article PubMed Google Scholar. An addition to these key-determinants, there are further Carbohydrate loading and muscle fatigue Meal prepping ideas that contribute to Fatigue and exercise performance psychophysiological state of an exercsie with Functional fitness training consequences anf perceived cognitive fatigue. Anf SAA, Lim J, Huettel SA. These findings indicate that prior handgrip exercise limited single-joint endurance performance of the knee extensors primarily by the interactions between perceived motor fatigue, effort perception, as well as affective valence and not by a decreased neuromuscular function. Telford RD, Catchpole EA, Deakin V, Hahn AG, Plank AW. Poor nutritional timing can also cause fatigue.

Exercuse has been Fatjgue differently in the literature performabce on the field of Fitness equipment online. The inconsistent use of an term fatigue Fatiigue scientific communication, thereby perfoemance progress towards a more Fatiigue understanding of dxercise phenomenon.

Therefore, Enoka and Duchateau Med Preformance Sports Exerc —38,[ Balancing sugar levels ] proposed a fatigue Fatigud that distinguishes between qnd fatigue i. Thereby, performance fatigability describes performane decrease in an Ftaigue performance measure, Fatiigue perceived fatigability refers to Fatiguue sensations that regulate the integrity of performsnce performer.

Although this framework served as a perfprmance starting point to unravel the psychophysiology Consistent hydration for sustained performance fatigue, several important Meal prepping ideas were not Fqtigue and the Fagigue of the mechanisms driving performance fatigability and perceived fatigability were not comprehensively discussed.

Fztigue, the present narrative review perforrmance to 1 update the fatigue framework suggested by Enoka Fztigue Duchateau Pergormance Sci Sports Fatiue —38,[ 3 ] pertaining the taxonomy i.

We propose to define motor or cognitive task-induced exrrcise fatigue as aFtigue psychophysiological condition characterized Family meal planning a decrease in motor or cognitive performance i. Exegcise dimensions are interdependent, hinge on different determinants, and depend on performancee homeostasis e.

Peeformance, there is no single factor performxnce determining performance fatigue and perceived fatigue in response Natural anti-hypertensive remedies motor or cognitive tasks, Meal prepping ideas. Fatgue, the ans weight of perforance determinant and their interaction are ;erformance by several exegcise.

Mitchell R. Smith, Chris Performxnce, … Aaron J. Kristy Martin, Romain Meeusen, performancf Ben Rattray. Jeroen Van Cutsem, Samuele Perfodmance, … Bart Roelands. Motor or cognitive task-induced state fatigue can be defined as a psychophysiological condition characterized by Fatiggue decrease in motor or cognitive performance i.

Performance fatigue Thermogenesis and exercise perceived fatigue are interdependent, hinge on Health-conscious energy source determinants, and depend on several modulating factors e.

The combined monitoring of performance fatigue and perfirmance fatigue measures as well as the investigation of the underlying mechanisms will Fatgue to unravel the interactions Meal prepping ideas the different dimensions of fatigue and their impact on human nad.

This will contribute Martial arts recovery drinks assess the relative weight of Meal prepping ideas determinant and their interactions depending on several performmance factors. The multitude of psychophysiological processes that inevitably accompany exercisf or cognitive activity above a Meal prepping ideas intensity or perfkrmance can become a limiting factor for motor as well as cognitive performance and are typically summarized under the umbrella term fatigue.

In perfofmance past, a variety of disciplines e. Due to this fragmentation, a multitude of Herbal weight loss tea recipe definitions emerged leading to an inconsistent use of the term and neglecting the perfromance interactions between the perfoormance psychophysiological adjustments and exericse resulting perceptual, affective, and cognitive responses.

Therefore, Ftaigue insights Fayigue the psychophysiological processes associated with fatigue preformance healthy and clinical populations as well as the development of effective interventions exercisee hampered Performamce 23 Digestive health and nutrient absorption. This exercuse not only crucial to increase Fatigke performance of athletes and healthy people, but it is wnd important for vulnerable, deconditioned, as well performanve clinical perfkrmance due to peerformance negative effects on the eprformance and cognitive capacity as Fatgiue as abd of life.

To resolve the Fatigur of fatigue definitions, Enoka and Duchateau [ 3 ] proposed a framework defining fatigue as a self-reported annd symptom esercise limits physical and cognitive functions due pergormance interactions between performance fatigability Fxtigue. Both performance perforkance and performmance fatigability depend on several factors that peeformance the decline exercisse motor performance i.

In their Functional fitness training, the authors highlighted the interdependence of performance fatigability and perceived fatigability with both Fatigue and exercise performance to the self-reported symptom exrrcise. The advantage exerccise this amd is its applicability exercixe both healthy and clinical exrrcise, since Fagigue refers Skin detoxification tips the fatigue mechanisms whose relative weight is subject- and task-dependent.

Although the African Mango seed blood sugar framework suggested by Exerccise and Duchateau [ 3 ] Turmeric soap benefits as a good starting point to unravel the psychophysiology of fatigue induced by motor and cognitive tasks, several important aspects were not included and discussed e.

Finally, the interdependence of the mechanisms driving performance fatigability and perceived fatigability as well as the need to thoroughly Fatgue Fatigue and exercise performance aspects were not comprehensively discussed.

Therefore, petformance present narrative review aimed at wxercise updating the exercize and definition of fatigue Fattigue by Enoka and Duchateau Fatigud 3 performabce pertaining the taxonomy i.

To annd define perfogmance, it is first important to differentiate between trait fatigue and state fatigue. Trait fatigue describes the fatigue experienced by Functional fitness training individual over a longer period rxercise time e.

Trait exedcise is a symptom associated with many diseases e. However, trait fatigue can also pfrformance present in a milder form in healthy people [ 9 ]. Activity-induced state fatigue, in turn, is characterized by an acute and temporary change in motor or cognitive performance as well as the subjective experience of weariness or exhaustion that occur in the context of a specific motor or cognitive task [ 37101112 ].

The acute reduction in motor and cognitive performance can be labeled as motor and cognitive performance fatigue, respectively. While motor performance fatigue e.

The motor and cognitive task-induced modulation of the perception of fatigue can be termed perceived motor and cognitive fatigue, respectively, which depend on the psychophysiological state of the individual. Motor and cognitive performance fatigue as well as perceived motor and cognitive fatigue further depend on factors related to body homeostasis, are interdependent, and hinge on different determinants Fig.

Thereby, the extent of motor and cognitive performance fatigue as well as perceived motor and cognitive fatigue depends on several modulating factors e. In the long term, this can result in a reduced quality of life, particularly in vulnerable, deconditioned, and clinical populations Fig.

The extent of state fatigue mirrored by these dimensions depends on several modulating factors b and can have negative consequences for the motor and cognitive capacity, which might negatively affect quality of life c particularly in vulnerable, deconditioned, and clinical populations.

The bidirectional arrows indicate the interdependence between all dimensions. Please note that effort perception, affective valence, self-regulation and self-control, as well as time perception were added to the potential determinants of perceived motor fatigue compared to the framework of Enoka and Duchateau [ 3 ].

Furthermore, cognitive performance fatigue, perceived cognitive fatigue, and the potentially contributing factors were added to the framework. CNS central nervous system,? unknown factors that should be added in the future.

Of note, this definition slightly differs from the taxonomy provided by Enoka and Duchateau [ 3 ], who have defined state fatigue as a self-reported disabling symptom derived from the interdependent attributes performance fatigability and perceived fatigability.

However, this definition introduces the problem that state fatigue is assessed by self-report, which is also reflected by quantifying perceived fatigability i. In addition, performance may decrease without a corresponding increase in the perception of fatigue or vice versa.

This potential selective change is not captured by the definition of Enoka and Duchateau [ 3 ]. In addition, since we do not refer to state fatigue as a self-reported disabling symptom, the term fatigability does not seem to be necessary as it does not contribute any benefit compared to the term fatigue.

In fact, the term fatigue was also formerly used to describe a decrease in performance e. The terms fatigability and fatigable could, however, be used synonymously as linguistic variations e.

Of note, in the following paragraphs, the proposed fatigue taxonomy was applied, even when the cited studies have not used this terminology. The psychophysiological alterations during fatiguing motor exercise can be interpreted as a protective mechanism that regulate exercise behavior to ensure the preservation of homeostasis of various physiological systems in the human body [ 151920 ].

This is in contrast to fatigue resulting from sustained cognitive tasks, the psychophysiological underpinnings of which remain unclear.

Motor performance fatigue traditionally termed muscle or neuromuscular fatigue can be quantified as a decrease in maximal voluntary force production capacity of the neuromuscular system, which is determined by neural and muscular factors [ 3 ].

Depending on the characteristics of the motor task e. To determine the origin of these changes within the neuromuscular system, a distinction between neural central and muscular peripheral determinants of motor performance fatigue has been established. Neural determinants include aspects related to muscle activation traditionally termed central fatigue that can change during a motor task Fig.

In this context, various processes play a role including the modulation of intrinsic properties of motoneurons, an increase in inhibitory afferent feedback from group III and IV muscle afferents, a decrease in facilitatory afferent feedback, and changes in neuromodulators [ 16 ].

In addition, activation patterns of synergistic and antagonistic muscles can change during a fatiguing motor task, which in turn can negatively affect intermuscular coordination and thus force production capacity [ 2728 ].

Beside these neural determinants, changes in the contractile function of muscles can contribute to the extent of motor performance fatigue Fig. The impairment of contractile function largely depends on muscle perfusion and the intramuscular metabolism.

For instance, intense motor tasks lead to an increased accumulation of metabolites e. Under physiological conditions, inorganic phosphate seems to be primarily responsible for the reduction in contractile function, while reactive oxygen species seems to be involved in the prolonged force depression after exercise [ 25262930 ].

Perceived motor fatigue refers to the increase in the subjective perception of fatigue emerging during a motor task that can affect motor task performance [ 513 ]. It is often defined as a transient sensation of tiredness, weariness, lack of energy, or exhaustion [ 3536 ].

Recently, it was proposed to define perceived fatigue as the feeling of a need to rest or a mismatch between effort expended and actual performance [ 36 ].

Irrespective of the specific definition, the nature and extent of perceived motor fatigue depend on the psychophysiological state of the individual, which shapes the perceptual, affective, and cognitive processes during exercise Fig.

For example, exercising above an individual critical threshold e. Therefore, an increased muscle activation signal is necessary to maintain the submaximal force output, which is associated with an increased effort perception [ 38 ]. Besides this, exercise-induced pain and discomfort arise as a result of the enhanced metabolite accumulation, breathing rate, and body temperature [ 39 ].

These perceptual responses to exercise make a person feel increasingly bad and require regulatory cognitive processes to avoid slowing down or stopping the motor task [ 54041 ].

Recently, Venhorst et al. It allows the classification of some of the determining factors of perceived motor fatigue into three dimensions.

Following this framework, the perceptual responses to exercise e. The intensity of these perceptions has an impact on 2 the affective-motivational dimension e. The motor task-induced changes in these dimensions strongly determine the processes in 3 the cognitive-evaluative dimension related to the decision to slow down or speed up pacing behavior or even to disengage from exercise.

These processes involve, for instance, self-regulation, self-control, and executive functioning Fig. This three-dimensional dynamical system framework allows the comprehensive as well as specific assessment of the factors determining perceived motor fatigue and contributes to the understanding of the strain-perception-thinking-action coupling during fatiguing exercise [ 5 ].

However, the interactions between the perceptual-discriminatory dimension, the affective-motivational dimension, and the cognitive-evaluative dimension should not be regarded as hierarchical but as interdependent.

Adapted three-dimensional dynamical system framework of perceived motor fatigue first proposed by Venhorst et al. The bidirectional arrows indicate the interdependence between the dimensions. The effort perceived during a motor task can be attributed to the perceptual-discriminatory dimension and is associated with perceived motor fatigue [ 1642 ].

Moreover, motor task-related effort perception is considered an important determinant of exercise behavior and endurance performance [ 54344 ]. Of note, there is controversy about whether effort perception results from centrally mediated feedforward mechanisms i.

However, it is well accepted that processing of sensory signals in the brain is involved [ 3845 ]. Effort perception, along with motivation, is one core element of the psychobiological model of endurance performance [ 4346 ] and it has been shown that interventions, which reduced effort perception during a sustained motor task, have subsequently led to an increased exercise tolerance e.

Conversely, effort perception during endurance exercise was higher and motor performance was reduced after interventions inducing homeostatic perturbations like hypoglycemia [ 34 ], hyperthermia [ 53 ], dehydration [ 54 ], hypoxia [ 55 ], and sleep deprivation [ 56 ]. For instance, intense motor tasks lead to the accumulation of metabolites in the extracellular environment, resulting in an increased exercise-induced muscle pain perception, due to the activation of group III and IV muscle afferents [ 39 ].

Acute interventions aiming to reduce exercise-induced muscle pain have been shown to improve performance during sustained submaximal motor tasks [ 57 ], whereas artificially increasing exercise-induced muscle pain had the opposite effect [ 58 ].

These examples provide evidence for the importance of exercise-induced perceptual responses e. The intensity of the perceptual responses e. The affective state of an individual also contributes to perceived motor fatigue and can influence exercise behavior as well as time to exhaustion during motor tasks [ 54259 ].

It is thought that ratings of affective valence and arousal can mirror the affective state of individuals. Affective valence reflects how a person currently feels in general i. These states are thought to be subjective indicators of the homeostatic status during motor tasks mediated by afferent nerve fibers that detect the mechanical, thermal, chemical, metabolic, and hormonal state of various tissues.

: Fatigue and exercise performance

References

Neuropharmacology ; 17 : — Morozova E, Yoo Y, Behrouzvaziri A, Zaretskaia M, Rusyniak D, Zaretsky D et al. Amphetamine enhances endurance by increasing heat dissipation. Physiol Rep ; 4 : e Graham TE, Spriet LL.

Performance and metabolic responses to a high caffeine dose during prolonged exercise. J Appl Physiol ; 71 : — Spriet LL, MacLean DA, Dyck DJ, Hultman E, Cederblad G, Graham TE.

Caffeine ingestion and muscle metabolism during prolonged exercise in humans. Am J Physiol ; : E—E Paluska SA. Caffeine and exercise. Curr Sports Med Rep ; 2 : — Magkos F, Kavouras SA. Caffeine use in sports, pharmacokinetics in man, and cellular mechanisms of action. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr ; 45 : — Jones G.

Caffeine and other sympathomimetic stimulants: modes of action and effects on sports performance. Essays Biochem ; 44 : — Young R, Glennon RA. Pharmacol BiochemBehav ; 66 : — Blinks JR, Olson CB, Jewell BR, Bravený P.

Influence of caffeine and other methylxanthines on mechanical properties of isolated mammalian heart muscle. Evidence for a dual mechanism of action.

Circ Res ; 30 : — Bell DG, Mclellan TM, Sabiston CM. Effect of ingesting caffeine and ephedrine on km run performance. Med Sci Sports Exerc ; 34 : Asai H, Asahi T, Yamamura M, Yamauchi-Kohno R, Saito A. Lack of behavioral tolerance by repeated treatment with taltirelin hydrate, a thyrotropin-releasing hormone analog, in rats.

Pharmacol Biochem Behav ; 82 : — Braiden RW, Fellingham GW, Conlee RK. Effects of cocaine on glycogen metabolism and endurance during high intensity exercise.

Med Sci Sports Exerc ; 26 : — Kim D. Practical use and risk of modafinil, a novel waking drug. Environ Health Toxicol ; 27 : e Jacobs I, Bell DG.

Effects of acute modafinil ingestion on exercise time to exhaustion. Med Sci Sports Exerc ; 36 : — Wu X, Fan W, Pan Y, Zhai Y, Niu Y, Li C et al. Synthesis, crystal structure and anti-fatigue effects of some benzamide derivatives.

Molecules ; 19 : — Qi B, Liu L, Zhang H, Zhou GX, Wang S, Duan XZ et al. Anti-fatigue effects of proteins isolated from Panax quinquefolium.

J Ethnopharmacol ; : — Bao L, Cai X, Wang J, Zhang Y, Sun B, Li Y. Anti-fatigue effects of small molecule oligopeptides isolated from Panax ginseng C. Meyer in mice. Nutrients ; 8 : Article PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar.

Qi B, Zhang L, Zhang Z, Ouyang J, Huang H. Effects of ginsenosides-Rb1 on exercise-induced oxidative stress in forced swimming mice. Pharmacogn Mag ; 10 : — Wang J, Li S, Fan Y, Chen Y, Liu D, Cheng H et al. Anti-fatigue activity of the water-soluble polysaccharides isolated from Panax ginseng CA Meyer.

Kim S, Kim J, Lee Y, Seo MK, Sung DJ. Anti-fatigue effects of acute red ginseng intake in recovery from repetitive anaerobic exercise.

Iran J Public Health ; 45 : — PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar. Liang MT, Podolka TD, Chuang WJ. Panax notoginseng supplementation enhances physical performance during endurance exercise.

Yong-xin X, Jian-jun Z. Evaluation of anti-fatigue activity of total saponins of Radix notoginseng. Indian J Med Res ; : — De Bock K, Eijnde BO, Ramaekers M, Hespel P.

Acute Rhodiola rosea intake can improve endurance exercise performance. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab ; 14 : — Kang DZ, Hong HD, Kim KI, Choi SY. Anti-fatigue effects of fermented rhodiola rosea extract in mice.

Prev Nutr Food Sci ; 20 : 38— Morihara N, Nishihama T, Ushijima M, Ide N, Takeda H, Hayama M. Garlic as an anti-fatigue agent. Mol Nutr Food Res ; 51 : — Morihara N, Ushijima M, Kashimoto N, Sumioka I, Nishihama T, Hayama M et al.

Aged garlic extract ameliorates physical fatigue. Biol Pharm Bull ; 29 : — Ushijima M, Sumioka I, Kakimoto M, Yokoyama K, Uda N, Matsuura H et al. Effect of garlic and garlic preparations on physiological and psychological stress in mice. Phytother Res ; 11 : — Verma SK, Rajeevan V, Jain P, Bordia A.

Effect of garlic Allium sativum oil on exercise tolerance in patients with coronary artery disease. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol ; 49 : — Lu YL, Chia CY, Liu YW, Hou WC. Biological activities and applications of dioscorins, the major tuber storage proteins of yam. J Tradit Complement Med ; 2 : 41— Wu Z, Zhang S, Li P, Lu X, Wang J, Zhao L et al.

Effect of aurantii fructus immaturus flavonoid on the contraction of isolated gastric smooth muscle strips in rats.

Evid Based Complement Alternat Med ; : Levic DS, Minkel JR, Wang WD, Rybski WM, Melville DB, Knapik EW. Animal model of Sar1b deficiency presents lipid absorption deficits similar to Anderson disease.

Tian HH, Ong WS, Tan CL. Nutritional supplement use among university athletes in Singapore. Singapore Med J ; 50 : — Lukaski HC. Vitamin and mineral status: effects on physical performance.

Nutrition ; 20 : — Brutsaert TD, Hernandez-Cordero S, Rivera J, Viola T, Hughes G, Haas JD. Iron supplementation improves progressive fatigue resistance during dynamic knee extensor exercise in iron-depleted, nonanemic women. Am J Clin Nutr ; 77 : — Telford RD, Catchpole EA, Deakin V, Hahn AG, Plank AW.

Int J Sport Nutr ; 2 : — Lai YA, Lin YA, Chang WH. Long-term effects of fish oil supplementation on exercise performance and anti-fatigue in college students: Board June 1, 00 AM— 30 PM. Med Sci Sport Exer ; 48 : 62— Wyss M, Kaddurah-Daouk R.

Creatine and creatinine metabolism. Physiol Rev ; 80 : — Anderson O. Creatine propels British athletes to Olympic gold medals: Is creatine the one true ergogenic aid. Running Res News ; 9 : 1—5. Google Scholar. Alford C, Cox H, Wescott R.

The effects of red bull energy drink on human performance and mood. Amino acids ; 21 : — Maughan RJ. Nutritional ergogenic aids and exercise performance. Nutr Res Rev ; 12 : — Download references. This study was supported by grants from the National Natural Science Foundation of China and to XL , and the National Science and Technology Major Project ZX09JC to XL.

Department of Pharmacology, School of Pharmacy, Second Military Medical University, Shanghai, China. You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar.

Correspondence to Xia Liu. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4. Reprints and permissions. Wan, Jj. et al. Muscle fatigue: general understanding and treatment. Exp Mol Med 49 , e Download citation. Received : 19 February Revised : 20 May Accepted : 23 May Published : 06 October Issue Date : October Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:.

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article. Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative.

European Journal of Applied Physiology Skip to main content Thank you for visiting nature. Download PDF. Subjects Diagnostic markers Drug development Molecularly targeted therapy.

Abstract Muscle fatigue is a common complaint in clinical practice. Introduction Fatigue is a common non-specific symptom experienced by many people and is associated with many health conditions. Factors that affect muscle contraction and fatigue The production of skeletal muscle force depends on contractile mechanisms, and failure at any of the sites upstream of the cross-bridges can contribute to the development of muscle fatigue, including nervous, ion, vascular and energy systems.

Neural contributions Central neurotransmitters, especially 5-HT, DA and NA, play important role during whole-body exercise and fatigue. Transcranial magnetic stimulation Transcranial magnetic stimulation involves applying magnetic stimulation to the motor cortex and is optimized to activate the muscle of interest.

Peripheral nerve high-intensity electrical stimulation High-intensity stimulation of the peripheral nerve directly activates the α-motoneuron, evoking a motor response m-wave from the muscle.

Biomarker for the diagnosis of muscle fatigue At present, there are still no specific factors that have been consistently associated with a particular type of fatigue.

Oxidative stress biomarkers Reactive oxygen species ROS remain at a low level in resting skeletal muscle but increase in response to contractile activity. Potential treatment for muscle fatigue Improper exercise, long time combat, military training and some related diseases for example, cancer and stroke can cause muscle fatigue, which negatively affects athletic achievement, military combat ability and patient recovery.

Synthetic products Amphetamine, ephedrine, caffeine, for example, are all synthetic products that excite the central nervous system or sympathetic nervous system and promote resistance to muscle fatigue.

Amphetamine Amphetamine is a phenethylamine-type stimulant and antidepressant that is highly addictive and produces euphoria and an elevated mood. Araliaceae ginseng species American ginseng, panax ginseng C.

Others Enhancing the energy metabolism effectively helps to improve exercise capacity. Nutritional supplements Several nutritional factors that may limit exercise performance have been identified, thus leading to the widespread use of nutritional strategies.

Red bull Red bull contains a mixture of carbohydrates, taurine, glucuronolactone, vitamin B and caffeine, and it is a commonly used energy drink. Others Carnitine plays an essential role in fatty acid oxidation in muscle.

Conclusions Muscle force production involves a sequence of events, extending from cortical excitation to motor unit activation to excitation—contraction coupling, and ultimately leading to muscle activation. References Gruet M, Temesi J, Rupp T, Levy P, Millet GY, Verges S.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Norheim KB, Jonsson G, Omdal R. Article CAS Google Scholar Silverman MN, Heim CM, Nater UM, Marques AH, Sternberg EM. Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Kroenke K, Wood DR, Mangelsdorff AD, Meier NJ, Powell JB. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Gandevia SC.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Bigland-Ritchie B, Jones DA, Hosking GP, Edwards RH. CAS PubMed Google Scholar Kent-Braun JA, Fitts RH, Christie A.

PubMed Google Scholar Swart J, Lamberts RP, Lambert MI, Lambert EV, Woolrich RW, Johnston S et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Klass M, Roelands B, Lévénez M, Fontenelle V, Pattyn N, Meeusen R et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Roelands B, Goekint M, Heyman E, Piacentini MF, Watson P, Hasegawa H et al.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Piacentini MF, Meeusen R, Buyse L, De SG, Kempenaers F, Van NJ et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Enoka RM, Fuglevand AJ. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Heckman CJ, Enoka RM. CAS PubMed Google Scholar Taylor JL, Amann M, Duchateau J, Meeusen R, Rice CL.

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Darques JL, Decherchi P, Jammes Y. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Darques JL, Jammes Y. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Vie B, Gomez N, Brerro-Saby C, Weber JP, Jammes Y.

Article PubMed Google Scholar Brerro-Saby C, Delliaux S, Steinberg JG, Jammes Y. Article PubMed Google Scholar Fichna J, Poole DP, Veldhuis N, MacEachern SJ, Saur D, Zakrzewski PK et al.

Article CAS Google Scholar MacIntosh BR, Holash RJ, Renaud JM. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Allen DG, Lamb GD, Westerblad H. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Wright JR, McCloskey DI, Fitzpatrick RC. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Degens H, Salmons S, Jarvis JC.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Pitcher JB, Miles TS. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Tachi M, Kouzaki M, Kanehisa H, Fukunaga T.

Article PubMed Google Scholar Degens H, Sanchez Horneros JM, Hopman MT. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Cole MA, Brown MD. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Lanza IR, Wigmore DM, Befroy DE, Kent-Braun JA. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Wigmore DM, Propert K, Kentbraun JA.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Amann M, Romer LM, Subudhi AW, Pegelow DF, Dempsey JA. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Amann M, Eldridge MW, Lovering AT, Stickland MK, Pegelow DF, Dempsey JA.

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Grassi B, Rossiter HB, Hogan MC, Howlett RA, Harris JE, Goodwin ML et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Kent JA, Ortenblad N, Hogan MC, Poole DC, Musch TI.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Homsher E. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Ashar FN, Moes A, Moore AZ, Grove ML, Chaves PHM, Coresh J et al.

Article CAS Google Scholar Nielsen J, Ortenblad N. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Ortenblad N, Westerblad H, Nielsen J. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Bergstrom J, Hermansen L, Hultman E, Saltin B.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Hermansen L, Hultman E, Saltin B. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Hargreaves M, McConell G, Proietto J. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Sahlin K, Tonkonogi M, Soderlund K. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Chin ER, Allen DG. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Nielsen J, Schroder HD, Rix CG, Ortenblad N.

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Ortenblad N, Nielsen J, Saltin B, Holmberg HC. Article PubMed CAS Google Scholar Metzger JM, Moss RL. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Pate E, Bhimani M, Franks-Skiba K, Cooke R.

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Stackhouse SK, Reisman DS, Binder-Macleod SA. CAS PubMed Google Scholar Westerblad H, Allen DG, Lannergren J.

CAS PubMed Google Scholar Allen DG, Trajanovska S. Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Debold EP. Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Delliaux S, Brerro-Saby C, Steinberg JG, Jammes Y.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Macdonald WA, Stephenson DG. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Panossian A, Wikman G.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Huey KA, Meador BM. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Jammes Y, Steinberg JG, Olivier M, Brerro-Saby C, Condo J, Ravailhe S et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Koh TJ.

Article PubMed Google Scholar Jiang B, Xiao W, Shi Y, Liu M, Xiao X. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Luo Z, Lei H, Sun Y, Liu X, Su DF. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Lei H, Sun Y, Luo Z, Yourek G, Gui H, Yang Y et al.

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Qin Z, Wan JJ, Sun Y, Wang PY, Su DF, Lei H et al. Article PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar Hoffman BW, Oya T, Carroll TJ, Cresswell AG. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar McNeil CJ, Martin PG, Gandevia SC, Taylor JL.

Article CAS Google Scholar Levenez M, Garland SJ, Klass M, Duchateau J. Article PubMed Google Scholar Patikas DA, Bassa H, Kotzamanidis C. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Papaiordanidou M, Guiraud D, Varray A.

Article PubMed Google Scholar Hwang IS, Huang CY, Wu PS, Chen YC, Wang CH. Article PubMed Google Scholar Kalmar JM, Del BC, Cafarelli E.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Behm DG, St-Pierre DM. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Kent-Braun JA. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Finsterer J.

Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Korzeniewski B. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Gosker HR, Schols AMWJ. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Malenfant S, Potus F, Fournier F, Breuils-Bonnet S, Pflieger A, Bourassa S et al.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Ding Y, Fang HQ, Shang W, Xiao Y, Sun T, Hou N et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Siegel AJ, Januzzi J, Sluss P, Lee-Lewandrowski E, Wood M, Shirey T et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Galal NM, Fouad HM, Saied A, Dabnon M. Article PubMed Google Scholar Esbjornsson M, Norman B, Suchdev S, Viru M, Lindhgren A, Jansson E.

Article CAS Google Scholar Speranza L, Grilli A, Patruno A, Franceschelli S, Felzani G, Pesce M et al. CAS Google Scholar Puig JG, Mateos FA, Miranda ME, Torres RJ, de Miguel E, Perez de Ayala C et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Bloomer RJ, Goldfarb AH, Wideman L, Mckenzie MJ, Consitt LA.

PubMed Google Scholar Margonis K, Fatouros IG, Jamurtas AZ, Nikolaidis MG, Douroudos L, Chatzinikolaou A et al. Article CAS Google Scholar Karsikas S, Myllymäki M, Heikkilä M, Sormunen R, Kivirikko KI, Myllyharju J et al. Article CAS Google Scholar Robertson JD, Maughan RJ, Duthie GG, Morrice PC.

Article CAS Google Scholar Ward WF, Qi W, Remmen HV, Zackert WE, Roberts LJ, Richardson A. Article PubMed Google Scholar Kawamoto EM, Vasconcelos AR, Degaspari S, Bohmer AE, Scavone C, Marcourakis T. CAS Google Scholar Pandey KB, Mehdi MM, Maurya PK, Rizvi SI.

Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Mendoza-Nunez VM, Ruiz-Ramos M, Sanchez-Rodriguez MA, Retana-Ugalde R, Munoz-Sanchez JL. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Cavalca V, Veglia F, Squellerio I, Marenzi G, Minardi F, De Metrio M et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Vitai M, Goth L.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Jha R, Rizvi SI. Article PubMed Google Scholar Li G, Liu L, Hu H, Zhao Q, Xie F, Chen K et al. CAS PubMed Google Scholar Palasuwan A, Suksom D, Margaritis I, Soogarun S, Rousseau AS. Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Rosety-Rodriguez M, Rosety M, Ordonez FJ.

Article Google Scholar Tan TY, Lu CH, Lin TK, Liou CW, Chuang YC, Schminke U. Article PubMed Google Scholar Basu S, Helmersson J, Jarosinska D, Sallsten G, Mazzolai B, Barregard L. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Nanda N, Bobby Z, Hamide A.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Kamper EF, Chatzigeorgiou A, Tsimpoukidi O, Kamper M, Dalla C, Pitychoutis PM et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Simpson RJ, Florida-James GD, Whyte GP, Guy K.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Speranza L, Grilli A, Patruno A, Franceschelli S, Felzani G, Pesce M et al. CAS PubMed Google Scholar Brandt C, Pedersen BK.

Article PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar Goetzl EJ, Huang MC, Kon J, Patel K, Schwartz JB, Fast K et al. Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar Stulnig T, Maczek C, Böck G, Majdic O, Wick G.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Foster AD, Soloviova K, Puliaeva I, Puliaiev M, Puliaev R, Finkelman F et al. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Nijs J, Van Oosterwijck J, Meeus M, Lambrecht L, Metzger K, Fremont M et al.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Nielsen AR, Mounier R, Plomgaard P, Mortensen OH, Penkowa M, Speerschneider T et al. Article CAS Google Scholar Finsterer J, Milvay E. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Weiss B, Laties VG. CAS PubMed Google Scholar Wyndham CH, Rogers GG, Benade AJ, Strydom NB.

CAS PubMed Google Scholar Gerald MC. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Morozova E, Yoo Y, Behrouzvaziri A, Zaretskaia M, Rusyniak D, Zaretsky D et al. Article PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar Graham TE, Spriet LL. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Spriet LL, MacLean DA, Dyck DJ, Hultman E, Cederblad G, Graham TE.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Paluska SA. Article PubMed Google Scholar Magkos F, Kavouras SA. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Jones G.

Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Young R, Glennon RA. Article CAS Google Scholar Blinks JR, Olson CB, Jewell BR, Bravený P. Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar Bell DG, Mclellan TM, Sabiston CM. Authors: Darías Holgado Email Darías Holgado.

Darías Holgado , Centro de Investigación, Mente, Cerebro y Comportamiento CIMCYC , Universidad de Granada; Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Universidad de Granada, Spain ORCID. Daniel Sanabria , Centro de Investigación, Mente, Cerebro y Comportamiento CIMCYC , Universidad de Granada; Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Universidad de Granada, Spain.

José C. Perales , Centro de Investigación, Mente, Cerebro y Comportamiento CIMCYC , Universidad de Granada; Departamento de Psicología Experimental, Universidad de Granada, Spain.

Miguel A. Similar to cognitive performance fatigue, it was proposed that perceived cognitive fatigue may arise as the consequence of the analysis of the costs and benefits of expending energy in a certain cognitive task [ ], and would depend on modification in neurotransmitter release [ 21 ].

Perceived cognitive fatigue would thus serve as a mechanism that would stop or change ongoing behavior when no longer beneficial. Of note, an alternative view on the origin and role of perceived cognitive fatigue is that of an anticipatory protection mechanism, akin to theories of motor task-induced fatigue [ , , ].

According to this view, perceived cognitive fatigue would act in anticipation of future functional alterations induced by prolonged task performance, to divert behavior away from the taxing activity [ 23 ]. It is assumed that several factors contribute to perceived cognitive fatigue.

As mentioned above, one of the most relevant factors thought to contribute to perceived cognitive fatigue is the cognitive or mental effort invested in the task [ 9 , ]. It is thought that cognitive effort is associated with cognitive control, meaning that non-automated cognitive control-dependent processes, like the execution of difficult cognitive tasks, require cognitive effort [ ].

As mentioned earlier, cognitive effort is perceived as costly as well as aversive and is only maintained or increased if it is expected to be beneficial.

The costs of prolonged cognitive effort investment comprise the intrinsic costs related to cognitive control allocation per se as well as the opportunity costs that arise from forgoing other more rewarding behavior [ , ].

However, there is also evidence that effort is not necessarily perceived as costly and can add value, meaning that the same outcome can be more rewarding when more and not less effort was invested [ ]. It has been shown that several brain areas are activated when cognitive control and effort are exerted, which include the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, anterior insula, lateral prefrontal cortex, and lateral parietal cortex [ , , , ].

Similar to the perceived effort induced by motor tasks, cognitive effort perception and objective measures of effort investment during the same task can be modulated by homeostatic perturbations such as sleep deprivation [ ] and heat stress [ ], respectively.

It was argued that the costs of effort have a considerable impact on motivation, which drives the behavior of humans [ 9 ].

This interaction is intuitive, since motivation is not only directed towards a specific goal but also refers to the intensity i. Müller and Apps [ 9 ] proposed that the psychophysiological processes associated with activity-induced state fatigue have an impact on motivation in two ways: they would cause direct changes in brain structures that motivate behaviors or would induce alterations in other systems, which are connected to or influenced by these brain areas.

Tasks that require sustained cognitive effort typically increase indices of sympathetic nervous system activity [ , , ], which is interpreted to reflect an aversive affective response [ ]. Indeed, it has been postulated that core affect, comprising affective valence pleasure-displeasure and arousal activation-deactivation , changes during sustained cognitive tasks.

Alternatively, 2 repeated conflicts and errors induce negative affective valence signaling that the current task is unrewarding. The latter is associated with perceived cognitive fatigue, which is thought to direct the individual to other more rewarding activities or to reduce effortful conflict monitoring, especially during externally mandated cognitive tasks [ ].

This view is in line with the notion that negative affect signalizes inadequate progress towards goal achievement [ ], which was also discussed in the context of perceived cognitive fatigue [ ].

Furthermore, it was proposed that the increasingly aversive sensation with time-on-task results from the effort-induced accumulation of opportunity costs that arise from forgoing other and more rewarding behavior [ 22 ].

Similar to motor tasks, performing sustained cognitive tasks requires self-regulation [ ], which describes the dynamic process of bringing thinking and behavior in line with the desired goal [ ]. During sustained cognitive tasks, individuals have to continuously self-regulate different aversive sensations e.

Self-regulation per se requires effort and relies on the integrity of executive functioning and in particular on the core executive functions, which can be classified into inhibitory control i. There are several ways in which people can self-regulate themselves to modify their sensations, feelings, thoughts, and behaviors in service of a personal goal including effortful self-control [ ].

Although self-regulation and self-control are often used interchangeably [ ], it was proposed that they refer to distinct processes [ 64 ]. While self-regulation refers to more general processes of goal-directed thoughts and behaviors, self-control can be defined as the process of overcoming predominant pre-potent, automatic response tendencies in favor of the desired goal [ 65 ].

Self-control is exerted during the execution of sustained cognitive tasks and requires motivation as well as attention [ 64 ]. This is in line with the view that performing a sustained cognitive task competes with motivational options e.

In addition to these key-determinants, there are further important aspects that contribute to the psychophysiological state of an individual with potential consequences for perceived cognitive fatigue.

For instance, it has been revealed that greater interest in a cognitive task resulted in less perceived cognitive fatigue despite a higher willingness to exert cognitive effort [ ]. In the same manner, it was argued that the level of controllability modulates the aversive responses and perceived cognitive fatigue during sustained cognitive activities [ ].

Furthermore, the execution of sustained cognitive tasks can be associated with changes in mood as well as with feelings like stress, anxiety, frustration, hopelessness, tension, and boredom [ , , , ]. Some of these were shown to be related to cognitive task performance [ ] and to modulate perceived cognitive fatigue [ 64 , , ].

For example, it was found that a task requiring the passive observation of strings of numbers resulted in higher boredom ratings, a steeper decline in affective valence, and higher perceived cognitive fatigue ratings compared to a cognitive task that consisted of adding three to each digit of a four-digit number [ ].

Interestingly, passively watching strings of numbers was also rated as effortful, which was interpreted as the effort to keep paying attention. It was also proposed that boredom, caused by the low intrinsic attractiveness of the task itself, can also be responsible for a decrease in cognitive task performance [ ], highlighting the interdependence between determinants of perceived cognitive fatigue and cognitive performance fatigue.

Moreover, it was assumed that expectations based on previous experiences might influence the psychophysiological state and the psychophysiological adjustments during sustained cognitive tasks [ ]. For instance, this was shown in response to heat stress [ , ], sleep deprivation [ ], and mouth rinsing with caffeine-maltodextrin [ ].

These sources of influence also highlight the similarity between the constructs of sleepiness and fatigue. Confusion between those concepts is very common and it remains unclear to what extent the subjective assessment measures allow researchers to clearly tease them apart [ ]. Besides these, there are further factors contributing to perceived cognitive fatigue that are increasingly studied and should be added to the list of potential determinants in the future.

Various modulating factors can influence the extent of cognitive performance fatigue and perceived cognitive fatigue Fig. The main subject-specific factors include age, sex, the existence of diseases, and cognitive fitness.

The extent of fatigue in the different domains is further determined by the characteristics of the cognitive task e. In the following sections, we will only discuss the most important subject- and cognitive task-specific factors that can modulate cognitive performance fatigue and perceived cognitive fatigue.

Cognitive abilities, such as processing speed and executive functioning, decline with advancing age due to structural and functional changes within the brain e.

It is thought that these age-related changes contribute to the increased compensatory brain activation observed during the execution of cognitive tasks [ ], which was assumed to accelerate cognitive performance fatigue development [ ].

However, the results of studies that have investigated the effect of age on cognitive performance fatigue are mixed and do not allow for a definite conclusion. For instance, it was found that reaction times remained constant in young and old adults after the execution of a working memory task performed for 60 min, while accuracy even increased in the elderly.

The authors proposed that the potential decline in cognitive performance was countered by the learning effect [ ].

This is in line with the results of Behrens et al. In contrast, Terentjeviene et al. However, reaction times and intra-individual variability of reaction times only increased in young males indicating higher cognitive performance fatigue compared to the older males. Since brain activation differs between sexes depending on the type of cognitive task [ , ], it might be assumed that the extent of cognitive performance fatigue after a sustained cognitive activity is different between males and females.

However, the results of experiments on that topic are inconclusive. For instance, there were no sex-differences in the error rate and reaction times, which remained constant over time, after performing a continuous performance test for 51 min [ ]. This is consistent with the results of Wang et al.

The authors did not observe differences in the number of errors and completed subtractions between males and females. However, the task lasted only 12 min and was possibly not long enough to induce cognitive performance fatigue.

In contrast to these studies, Noreika et al. They have revealed that accuracy increased, and response times decreased to a larger extent in males compared to females with time-on-task.

However, since cognitive performance increased over time, these results did not indicate cognitive performance fatigue and might be biased by the learning or practice effect.

Since the extent of cognitive performance fatigue strongly depends on the structural and functional integrity of the central nervous system, it might be assumed that cognitive performance fatigue development is accelerated in patient populations, especially in those with neurological diseases affecting the central nervous system.

However, studies investigating cognitive performance fatigue in different patient populations e. Despite similar increases in cognitive task performance, patients showed heightened cerebral activation in specific areas and an increased perceived cognitive fatigue [ 10 , , ] indicating a lower efficiency.

In contrast, a decrease in cognitive performance measures was revealed during the execution of a four-block paced auditory serial addition test with an earlier drop in performance in people with multiple sclerosis compared to healthy controls [ ].

Analogous results were obtained by other studies that investigated the effects of sustained cognitive tasks on cognitive performance fatigue in multiple sclerosis and healthy controls [ , ]. A decrease in cognitive task performance was also found in stroke survivors while performing a min inhibition task, which was, however, comparable to the cognitive task performance reduction of the healthy control group [ ].

Moreover, Jordan et al. The discrepant findings presented above indicate that the effect of age, sex, and diseases on cognitive performance fatigue seems to be strongly influenced by the task characteristics e.

The occurrence and evaluation of cognitive performance fatigue strongly depend on the type of task e. For instance, Smith et al. While the first task required only vigilance, the other two tasks additionally relied on response inhibition.

Cognitive performance i. Interestingly, cognitive performance monitored during the respective tasks deteriorated only for the psychomotor vigilance task i. In contrast, pre- and post-cognitive performance assessments with the 3-min psychomotor vigilance task revealed increased reaction times only after the AX-continuous performance task and the Stroop task.

These data indicate that the detection and extent of cognitive performance fatigue strongly depend on the type of task as well as the task used to assess the potential change in performance.

Besides the effect of the type of cognitive task, other task characteristics e. Although several studies have not found a decline in cognitive performance with time-on-task, it was demonstrated that cognitive task performance decreased with increasing task duration [ 21 , , ]. Furthermore, it was proposed that the cognitive load induced by the task determines the extent of cognitive performance fatigue [ , , ].

Studies on this topic manipulated the cognitive load either by increasing the difficulty of the task e. However, the results of these studies are inconsistent. For instance, during a min working memory task, Shigihara et al.

The authors have additionally tested the effect of the sustained cognitive tasks on the advanced trail making test performance and have found that the number of errors increased from pre to post for both conditions. Moreover, the results of Borragàn et al. However, irrespective of the task characteristics, there is a general critique that laboratory cognitive tasks are not ecologically valid and exhibit low intrinsic motivational value.

Therefore, controllability of these tasks is low, people experience them as increasingly aversive, and might disengage from the tasks because they are not sufficiently important for them [ 22 , 64 , , ]. Subject-specific factors like age, sex, and the presence of diseases might also modulate perceived cognitive fatigue and its potential determinants.

For example, Wascher et al. Interestingly, young adults showed also a concomitant larger decrease in self-reported motivation. This is in line with the results of Terentjeviene et al. This was accompanied by a higher perceived cognitive effort and temporal demand in the young participants during task execution.

The young adults additionally reported increases in tension and confusion as well as a decrease in vigor, which were not found for the older participants.

These data collectively suggest that younger adults perceive sustained cognitive tasks as more effortful, demanding, and fatiguing than older adults, which is corroborated by the larger decreases in motivation and vigor as well as the increased tension and confusion.

Since laboratory cognitive tasks are often performed using a computer and older adults have less experience with digital technology [ ], these differences might be related to the higher intrinsic attractiveness of the task for older people.

Nevertheless, it was also shown that older people with a low frequency of technology use have higher levels of computer anxiety [ ]. Contrary to the findings of age-related differences, it has also been observed that the increase in perceived cognitive fatigue induced by a min inhibitory control task was not different between young and old adults [ 11 ].

Studies on sex-differences in perceived cognitive fatigue also revealed partially inconsistent results. Some studies have not found differences in perceived cognitive fatigue between males and females after performing a min Stroop task and a min continuous performance test, which require response inhibition and sustained attention.

Similarly, no sex-differences in the changes in cognitive effort, vigor, energy, tiredness, tension, calmness, and further self-reported data recorded during these tasks were reported [ , ]. Nevertheless, others have observed higher increases in fatigue ratings during a min mental rotation task in females compared to males, which depended on the menstrual cycle phase [ ].

It was additionally revealed that perceived cognitive effort and perceived task difficulty were higher in females compared to males during both a high and low cognitive load condition involving arithmetic tasks [ ].

There is evidence that perceived cognitive fatigue development in response to sustained cognitive tasks is higher in some diseases, especially those affecting the central nervous system [ 10 , , , ].

Higher increases in fatigue ratings during cognitive tasks have particularly been observed in people with multiple sclerosis [ , , ]. It was further reported that the perceived workload e. In contrast, studies on other patient populations, such as people with chronic fatigue syndrome, depression, and myasthenia gravis, have not found differential changes in perceived cognitive fatigue ratings compared with healthy controls during sustained cognitive tasks [ , ].

It has been shown that perceived cognitive fatigue and its determinants can be influenced by the type of task e. The results of studies that have examined the effect of the type of cognitive task on perceived cognitive fatigue are inconclusive.

The latter was applied with constant processing intervals and with shorter individualized processing intervals based on the maximal performance determined during an incremental TloadDback task.

Although the task duration differed greatly, all cognitive tasks induced perceived cognitive fatigue, assessed with a visual analog scale, with the highest increase in the individualized TloadDback task condition.

However, the AX continuous performance test induced a higher increase in perceived cognitive fatigue and a larger decrease in vigor assessed with the Brunel Mood Scale. These results were accompanied by higher sleepiness ratings and a larger drop in task motivation compared to the TloadDback task condition.

Another study on this topic has compared perceived cognitive fatigue as well as its recovery in response to a psychomotor vigilance task, an AX-continuous performance task, and a Stroop task each performed for 45 min [ ]. The tasks differed regarding their demands on vigilance and response inhibition.

Although all tasks increased perceived cognitive fatigue, the authors concluded that tasks requiring response inhibition appeared to induce perceived cognitive fatigue for a longer duration than a simple vigilance task. With regard to other task characteristics, it was often found that perceived cognitive fatigue progressively increases with time-on-task [ , ].

Moreover, it was shown that altering the cognitive load by the difficulty of the task i. In line with this, Borragàn et al. However, when cognitive load was modulated by decreasing the processing interval for the stimuli, the high cognitive load condition induced a higher increase in perceived cognitive fatigue compared to the low cognitive load condition.

The authors concluded that the processing time interval during cognitive tasks is more relevant for perceived cognitive fatigue development than the number of processed items. In contrast to this, it was also shown that perceived cognitive fatigue cannot only result from performing a sustained cognitive task i.

More specifically, perceived cognitive fatigue was even higher in the boredom condition compared with the cognitive task condition despite lower cognitive effort ratings. This might be related to the steeper decline in affective valence ratings and the lower task interest ratings in the boredom condition.

Indeed, higher interest in a task has been shown to induce less perceived cognitive fatigue [ ]. These data highlight the importance of the characteristics of the cognitive task for the development of perceived cognitive fatigue and its determinants.

The updated framework covers the different dimensions of task-induced state fatigue and the involved mechanisms Fig. Thereby, the interdependence of performance fatigue and perceived fatigue as well as their determinants is acknowledged and highlighted.

There is no single factor primarily determining performance fatigue and perceived fatigue in response to motor and cognitive tasks. Instead, the relative weight of each determinant and their interaction depends on several modulating factors e.

Although the mechanisms of motor performance fatigue are not yet fully elucidated, there are extensive data on the changes in the nervous system and muscle during motor tasks contributing to the decline in motor performance [ 16 , 17 , 25 , 26 ].

In contrast, the mechanisms underlying perceived motor fatigue and their interactions with motor performance fatigue received less attention. Therefore, future research should not only investigate the neural and muscular mechanisms driving motor performance fatigue but also aspects of perceived motor fatigue and the corresponding neuro physiological correlates in detail.

This approach can assist in investigating the motor task-induced perceptual differences between individuals and exercise protocols e. This is of special importance for clinical populations suffering from an increased prevalence of motor performance fatigue and perceived motor fatigue e.

There are a few studies available that have assessed neural and muscular contributions to motor performance fatigue in parallel with ratings of perceived motor fatigue, effort perception, and affective valence to study their interactions [ 42 , ]. For instance, Greenhouse-Tucknott et al.

They have shown that prior submaximal hand grip exercise reduced the time to exhaustion during a submaximal isometric contraction of the knee extensors without altering neuromuscular function. Thereby, effort perception and affective valence were correlated with time to exhaustion and the ratings of perceived motor fatigue.

These findings indicate that prior handgrip exercise limited single-joint endurance performance of the knee extensors primarily by the interactions between perceived motor fatigue, effort perception, as well as affective valence and not by a decreased neuromuscular function.

Similar approaches should be adopted in the future to investigate the interactions between motor performance fatigue and perceived motor fatigue in different populations, particularly in those suffering from diseases.

Besides the combined investigation of motor performance fatigue, perceived motor fatigue, and the underlying mechanisms, the determining factors can be manipulated to elucidate their causal involvement in the development of state fatigue in different populations and in response to various motor tasks.

For that purpose, different interventions can be used aiming to modify the physiological and psychological regulatory processes during fatiguing motor exercise. For instance, neuromodulation techniques like tDCS are suitable to alter cortical excitability and to investigate the effects of changed neural properties on motor performance fatigue and perceived motor fatigue [ 67 ].

Furthermore, other interventions can be applied to modify neural as well as muscular properties e. Interventions aiming to modulate the psychological determinants of endurance performance have also been shown to induce changes in motor performance and the perceptual responses to fatiguing exercise [ 48 ].

These strategies could be used to investigate the role of cognitive processes in the interpretation of perceptual responses and the change in affective valence emerging during fatiguing motor exercise.

Motor performance fatigue can be assessed using maximal and submaximal motor performance measures. Maximal performance tasks e. In addition, the variation of submaximal motor performance is also an indication of motor performance fatigue e. The neural and muscular mechanisms contributing to motor performance fatigue can be investigated with different non-invasive techniques.

Neural adjustments i. Moreover, functional magnetic resonance imaging is suitable to monitor changes within cortical and subcortical structures during motor exercise [ ]. The contractile function of muscles can be validly quantified using peripheral nerve stimulation [ ], while changes in muscle oxygenation and muscle metabolism can be measured with near-infrared spectroscopy and phosphorus magnetic resonance spectroscopy, respectively [ , ].

In addition to these measures, perceived motor fatigue as well as the contributing factors should be comprehensively assessed before, during, and after fatiguing exercise. For this purpose, different scales and questionnaires can be used according to the focus of the respective study.

These measures should be applied together with standardized wording as described elsewhere [ 5 , 38 ]. Furthermore, the attentional focus during fatiguing motor exercise should be recorded as an index for the motor task intensity-dependent attentional shift from an external focus on the surrounding to an internal focus on the bodily sensations [ ].

These aspects should be quantified in conjunction with affective valence and arousal, recorded with the feeling scale and felt arousal scale, respectively, as indicators of the motor task-dependent homeostatic perturbations [ 40 , 60 ].

It has been shown that these aspects can influence perceived motor fatigue and performance during fatiguing motor tasks. Moreover, they reflect the motor task-induced homeostatic perturbations in various physiological subsystems and are thus indicators of the physical demands.

Besides these core measures of perceived motor fatigue, additional scales and tests should be used according to the aim of the respective study.

This should be done to investigate the role of self-regulation capacity, executive functioning [ 41 , , ], and other determinants for motor performance fatigue as well as perceived motor fatigue. The interactions between cognitive performance fatigue and perceived cognitive fatigue have been investigated more comprehensively and in greater detail compared to those between motor performance fatigue and perceived motor fatigue.

Accordingly, many studies in this field have recorded both changes in cognitive performance as well as in the perception of fatigue during and after sustained cognitive tasks. However, it must be pointed out that in studies which have measured cognitive performance on the same task as the one used to induce fatigue, evidence for a decline in cognitive task performance was frequently missing [ , ].

The lack of a systematic decline in cognitive performance with time-on-task was often attributed to an increased compensatory cognitive effort or to a learning effect that would lead to a performance increase overcoming the performance decline induced by fatigue [ , , , ].

In contrast, increases in perceived cognitive fatigue with time-on-task have been shown very consistently across many different conditions e.

Therefore, future studies should use separate cognitive tasks to induce and measure cognitive performance fatigue as already done by some studies [ 23 , 24 , , , ]. This approach might bypass the influence of a decreased motivation or learning effect and has typically led to more consistent correlations between the cognitive performance decline and perceived cognitive fatigue [ 24 , ].

Furthermore, the effect of distinct types e. There is evidence that different cognitive tasks induce specific declines in performance measures depending on the assessment task. On the contrary, pre and post cognitive performance assessments with a 3-min psychomotor vigilance task revealed only increased reaction times after the AX-continuous performance task and the Stroop task.

However, perceived cognitive fatigue increased in all conditions, even though it tended to persist longer after the tasks requiring more response inhibition.

Moreover, cognitive performance fatigue and perceived cognitive fatigue measures were also shown to be sensitive to the manipulation of the cognitive load [ , ]. Consequently, future studies on this topic should not only examine the effects of diverse types of tasks on cognitive performance fatigue, perceived cognitive fatigue, and their neural correlates but also the influence of varying cognitive loads.

Furthermore, it is likely that the level of overlap between the fatiguing and the assessment tasks, in terms of the cognitive processes involved, is also crucial [ ]. Therefore, it appears essential that future studies assess performance before and after the fatiguing cognitive task not only with cognitive tasks requiring similar cognitive processes, but also with tasks that involve different processes.

These experiments should further take the impact of the cognitive load, the nature of the cognitive processes involved, and the degree of process overlap with the fatiguing task into account. As stated above, other important sources of influence are mood and emotional variables, like stress, anxiety, frustration, hopelessness, tension, and boredom [ , , , ], which were shown to modulate perceived cognitive fatigue [ , , ] and cognitive task performance [ ].

Therefore, it seems mandatory to quantify these aspects and to analyze their effects on cognitive performance fatigue, perceived cognitive fatigue, and their neural correlates.

Due to the discrepant findings, the influence of subject-specific factors i. Nevertheless, it seems that cognitive task-induced perceived cognitive fatigue is higher in some patient populations [ , , ].

To address the causal relationships, neurophysiological and psychophysiological determinants of cognitive performance fatigue as well as perceived cognitive fatigue can also be modulated experimentally.

For instance, it was shown that anodal tDCS applied to the right parietal cortex counteracted the cognitive performance decline during a min visual vigilance task in healthy controls and people with multiple sclerosis but had no effect on perceived cognitive fatigue [ ].

Similar effects were observed in multiple sclerosis patients after stimulating the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex with anodal tDCS [ ]. In addition, investigating the effects of different neuromodulatory substances such as caffeine on cognitive performance fatigue, perceived cognitive fatigue [ ], and its neurophysiological correlates can help to unravel their interdependence.

As outlined above, the detection and quantification of cognitive performance fatigue strongly depend on the assessment task and the considered variables e. These measures should be combined with techniques suitable to record brain activity e. Ample evidence points to the interactions between the different dimensions of activity-induced state fatigue.

For instance, Marcora et al. However, effort perception during exercise was higher after performing the sustained cognitive task leading the authors to the conclusion that the participants reached their maximal tolerable effort level earlier and subsequently disengaged from exercise.

These data indicate that fatiguing cognitive or motor activities seem to modulate the performance and perceptions during a subsequent fatiguing motor or cognitive task, respectively.

Evidence for the interactions between the different dimensions of activity-induced state fatigue also arises from experiments that have investigated the psychophysiological adjustments and performance changes in response to sustained motor-cognitive dual tasks [ , , ].

These studies have found a decreased time to exhaustion during submaximal motor tasks when a concurrent cognitive task had to be executed e. Additionally, time to exhaustion during a fatiguing motor-cognitive dual task tended to be shorter for a high compared to a low cognitive load condition.

This was accompanied by a higher reduction in muscle activation of the knee extensors i. As expected, cognitive effort perception scaled with the level of cognitive load, but, surprisingly, effort perception associated with the motor task was greater in the high cognitive load condition compared to that recorded during the single motor task condition [ ].

These data have impressively shown that the different domains of activity-induced state fatigue interact with each other. Since there is an overlap of brain structures involved during the execution of fatiguing motor and cognitive tasks e.

Thereby, the degree of overlap between cognitive processes required for the respective motor and cognitive task might mediate the detrimental effects on performance and perceptions [ 9 , , ].

Consequently, future studies should investigate the effects of diverse types e. These effects should also be studied in relation to various motor tasks e.

Conversely, the impact of different fatiguing motor tasks performed prior to various fatiguing cognitive tasks relying on distinct cognitive processes should be examined.

The mechanistic basis for the interactions between motor performance fatigue, perceived motor fatigue, cognitive performance fatigue, and perceived cognitive fatigue can be probed with the methods mentioned in the respective paragraphs above. However, the quantification of some perceptual responses to motor and cognitive tasks requires specific scales and a clear description with standardized wording.

This is of particular importance for the measurement of perceived fatigue and effort in response to motor, cognitive, or motor-cognitive dual tasks. Although it was proposed that the feeling of fatigue arising from exertion might be similarly induced by motor and cognitive tasks [ 9 ], results of studies indicated that perceived motor fatigue and perceived cognitive fatigue represent different perceptual domains [ , ].

The same applies to effort perception, which can be related to either to the motor or the cognitive task [ ]. Performance fatigue and perceived fatigue as well as their determinants are interdependent and should not be considered in isolation.

Consequently, there is no single factor primarily determining performance fatigue and perceived fatigue in response to motor and cognitive tasks. Instead, the relative weight of each determinant and their interaction depend on body homeostasis e. Therefore, a combined assessment of performance fatigue and perceived fatigue measures as well as its neuro physiological correlates is required to unravel the psychophysiology of motor and cognitive task-induced state fatigue.

This will help to better understand the interactions between the different dimensions of fatigue and their impact on human performance, which is necessary to design effective interventions for increasing exercise tolerance and human performance in healthy and clinical populations.

Enoka RM, Duchateau J. Muscle fatigue: what, why and how it influences muscle function. J Physiol. Article CAS Google Scholar. Kluger BM, Krupp LB, Enoka RM. Fatigue and fatigability in neurologic illnesses: proposal for a unified taxonomy. Article Google Scholar. Translating fatigue to human performance.

Med Sci Sports Exerc. Tommasin S, de Luca F, Ferrante I, Gurreri F, Castelli L, Ruggieri S, et al. Cognitive fatigability is a quantifiable distinct phenomenon in multiple sclerosis. J Neuropsychol. Venhorst A, Micklewright D, Noakes TD. Perceived fatigability: utility of a three-dimensional dynamical systems framework to better understand the psychophysiological regulation of goal-directed exercise behaviour.

Sports Med. Braley TJ, Chervin RD. Fatigue in multiple sclerosis: mechanisms, evaluation, and treatment. Gruet M. Fatigue in chronic respiratory diseases: theoretical framework and implications for real-life performance and rehabilitation. Front Physiol. Marrelli K, Cheng AJ, Brophy JD, Power GA.

Perceived versus performance fatigability in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Müller T, Apps MAJ. Motivational fatigue: a neurocognitive framework for the impact of effortful exertion on subsequent motivation. Genova HM, Rajagopalan V, Deluca J, Das A, Binder A, Arjunan A, et al.

Examination of cognitive fatigue in multiple sclerosis using functional magnetic resonance imaging and diffusion tensor imaging. PLoS ONE. Behrens M, Mau-Moeller A, Lischke A, Katlun F, Gube M, Zschorlich V, et al. Mental fatigue increases gait variability during dual-task walking in old adults.

J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. Behrens M, Broscheid K-C, Schega L. Taxonomie und Determinanten motorischer performance fatigability bei Multipler Sklerose. Micklewright D, St Clair Gibson A, Gladwell V, Al Salman A. Development and validity of the rating-of-fatigue scale.

Gandevia SC. Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol Rev. Noakes TD. Fatigue is a brain-derived emotion that regulates the exercise behavior to ensure the protection of whole-body homeostasis. Taylor JL, Amann M, Duchateau J, Meeusen R, Rice CL.

Neural contributions to muscle fatigue: from the brain to the muscle and back again. Taylor JL, Gandevia SC. A comparison of central aspects of fatigue in submaximal and maximal voluntary contractions.

J Appl Physiol. Marcora SM, Staiano W, Manning V. Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. Blain GM, Mangum TS, Sidhu SK, Weavil JC, Hureau TJ, Jessop JE, et al. Laurin J, Pertici V, Dousset E, Marqueste T, Decherchi P.

Group III and IV muscle afferents: role on central motor drive and clinical implications. Boksem MAS, Tops M. Mental fatigue: costs and benefits. Brain Res Rev.

Kurzban R. The sense of effort. Curr Opin Psychol. Benoit C-E, Solopchuk O, Borragán G, Carbonnelle A, van Durme S, Zénon A. Cognitive task avoidance correlates with fatigue-induced performance decrement but not with subjective fatigue. Gergelyfi M, Sanz-Arigita EJ, Solopchuk O, Dricot L, Jacob B, Zénon A.

Mental fatigue correlates with depression of task-related network and augmented DMN activity but spares the reward circuit. Allen DG, Lamb GD, Westerblad H. Skeletal muscle fatigue: cellular mechanisms.

Cheng AJ, Place N, Westerblad H. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. Ebenbichler GR, Kollmitzer J, Glckler L, Bochdansky T, Kopf A, Fialka V. The role of the biarticular agonist and cocontracting antagonist pair in isometric muscle fatigue.

Muscle Nerve. Gagnon D, Bertrand Arsenault A, Smyth G, Kemp F. Cocontraction changes in muscular fatigue at different levels of isometric contraction.

Int J Ind Ergon. Allen DG, Clugston E, Petersen Y, Röder IV, Chapman B, Rudolf R. Interactions between intracellular calcium and phosphate in intact mouse muscle during fatigue.

Westerblad H. Acidosis is not a significant cause of skeletal muscle fatigue. Hunter SK. Performance fatigability: mechanisms and task specificity. Nybo L, Rasmussen P, Sawka MN.

Performance in the heat-physiological factors of importance for hyperthermia-induced fatigue. Compr Physiol. Goodall S, González-Alonso J, Ali L, Ross EZ, Romer LM.

Supraspinal fatigue after normoxic and hypoxic exercise in humans. Nybo L. CNS fatigue and prolonged exercise: effect of glucose supplementation. Vargas NT, Marino F. A neuroinflammatory model for acute fatigue during exercise. Skau S, Sundberg K, Kuhn H-G. A proposal for a unifying set of definitions of fatigue.

Front Psychol. Jones AM, Burnley M, Black MI, Poole DC, Vanhatalo A. Pageaux B. Perception of effort in exercise science: definition, measurement and perspectives. Eur J Sport Sci.

Mauger AR. Fatigue is a pain-the use of novel neurophysiological techniques to understand the fatigue—pain relationship. Ekkekakis P, Hall EE, Petruzzello SJ. Variation and homogeneity in affective responses to physical activity of varying intensities: an alternative perspective on dose-response based on evolutionary considerations.

J Sports Sci. Hyland-Monks R, Cronin L, McNaughton L, Marchant D. The role of executive function in the self-regulation of endurance performance: a critical review. Prog Brain Res. Greenhouse-Tucknott A, Wrightson JG, Raynsford M, Harrison NA, Dekerle J. Interactions between perceptions of fatigue, effort, and affect decrease knee extensor endurance performance following upper body motor activity, independent of changes in neuromuscular function.

Marcora SM. Do we really need a central governor to explain brain regulation of exercise performance? Eur J Appl Physiol. Staiano W, Bosio A, de Morree HM, Rampinini E, Marcora S. The cardinal exercise stopper: muscle fatigue, muscle pain or perception of effort?

Marcora S. Perception of effort during exercise is independent of afferent feedback from skeletal muscles, heart, and lungs. Marcora SM, Staiano W. The limit to exercise tolerance in humans: mind over muscle? Doherty M, Smith PM. Effects of caffeine ingestion on rating of perceived exertion during and after exercise: a meta-analysis.

Scand J Med Sci Sports. McCormick A, Meijen C, Marcora S. Psychological determinants of whole-body endurance performance. Terry PC, Karageorghis CI, Curran ML, Martin OV, Parsons-Smith RL.

Effects of music in exercise and sport: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. Smirmaul BPC, de Moraes AC, Angius L, Marcora SM.

Muscle fatigue: general understanding and treatment

No effect of a noradrenergic reuptake inhibitor on performance in trained cyclists. Med Sci Sports Exerc ; 34 : — Enoka RM, Fuglevand AJ. Motor unit physiology: some unresolved issues. Muscle nerve ; 24 : 4— Heckman CJ, Enoka RM.

Motor unit. Taylor JL, Amann M, Duchateau J, Meeusen R, Rice CL. Neural contributions to muscle fatigue: from the brain to the muscle and back again. Med Sci Sports Exerc ; 48 : — Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar.

Darques JL, Decherchi P, Jammes Y. Mechanisms of fatigue-induced activation of group IV muscle afferents: the roles played by lactic acid and inflammatory mediators. Neurosci Lett ; : — Darques JL, Jammes Y. Fatigue-induced changes in group IV muscle afferent activity: differences between high- and low-frequency electrically induced fatigues.

Brain Res ; : — Vie B, Gomez N, Brerro-Saby C, Weber JP, Jammes Y. Changes in stationary upright standing and proprioceptive reflex control of foot muscles after fatiguing static foot inversion.

J Biomech ; 46 : — Article PubMed Google Scholar. Brerro-Saby C, Delliaux S, Steinberg JG, Jammes Y. Fatigue-induced changes in tonic vibration response TVR in humans: relationships between electromyographic and biochemical events. Muscle Nerve ; 38 : — Fichna J, Poole DP, Veldhuis N, MacEachern SJ, Saur D, Zakrzewski PK et al.

Transient receptor potential vanilloid 4 inhibits mouse colonic motility by activating NO-dependent enteric neurotransmission. J Mol Med Berl ; 93 : — MacIntosh BR, Holash RJ, Renaud JM. Skeletal muscle fatigue—regulation of excitation-contraction coupling to avoid metabolic catastrophe.

J Cell Sci ; : — Allen DG, Lamb GD, Westerblad H. Impaired calcium release during fatigue. Wright JR, McCloskey DI, Fitzpatrick RC. Effect of muscle perfusion on fatigue andsystemic arterial pressure i human subjects.

J Appl Physiol ; 86 : — Degens H, Salmons S, Jarvis JC. Intramuscular pressure, force and blood flow in rabbit tibialis anterior muscles during single and repetitive contractions. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol ; 78 : 13— Pitcher JB, Miles TS.

Influence of muscle blood flow on fatigue during intermittent human hand-grip exercise and recovery. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol ; 24 : — Tachi M, Kouzaki M, Kanehisa H, Fukunaga T. The influence of circulatory difference on muscle oxygenation and fatigue during intermittent static dorsiflexion.

Eur J Appl Physiol ; 91 : — Degens H, Sanchez Horneros JM, Hopman MT. Acute hypoxia limits endurance but does not affect muscle contractile properties.

Muscle Nerve ; 33 : — Cole MA, Brown MD. Response of the human triceps surae muscle to electrical stimulation during varying levels of blood flow restriction. Eur J Appl Physiol ; 82 : 39— Lanza IR, Wigmore DM, Befroy DE, Kent-Braun JA.

In vivo ATP production during free-flow and ischaemic muscle contractions in humans. J Physiol ; : — Wigmore DM, Propert K, Kentbraun JA. Blood flow does not limit skeletal muscle force production during incremental isometric contractions.

Eur J Appl Physiol ; 96 : — Amann M, Romer LM, Subudhi AW, Pegelow DF, Dempsey JA. Severity of arterial hypoxaemia affects the relative contributions of peripheral muscle fatigue to exercise performance in healthy humans.

Amann M, Eldridge MW, Lovering AT, Stickland MK, Pegelow DF, Dempsey JA. Arterial oxygenation influences central motor output and exercise performance via effects on peripheral locomotor muscle fatigue in humans. Grassi B, Rossiter HB, Hogan MC, Howlett RA, Harris JE, Goodwin ML et al.

Faster O 2 uptake kinetics in canine skeletal muscle in situ after acute creatine kinase inhibition. Kent JA, Ortenblad N, Hogan MC, Poole DC, Musch TI. No Muscle Is an Island: Integrative Perspectives on Muscle Fatigue. Homsher E. Muscle enthalpy production and its relationship to actomyosin ATPase.

Annu Rev Physiol ; 49 : — Ashar FN, Moes A, Moore AZ, Grove ML, Chaves PHM, Coresh J et al. Association of mitochondrial DNA levels with frailty and all-cause mortality.

Nielsen J, Ortenblad N. Physiological aspects of the subcellular localization of glycogen in skeletal muscle. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab ; 38 : 91— Ortenblad N, Westerblad H, Nielsen J. Muscle glycogen stores and fatigue.

Bergstrom J, Hermansen L, Hultman E, Saltin B. Diet, muscle glycogen and physical performance. Acta Physiol Scand ; 71 : — Hermansen L, Hultman E, Saltin B. Muscle glycogen during prolonged severe exercise.

Hargreaves M, McConell G, Proietto J. Influence of muscle glycogen on glycogenolysis and glucose uptake during exercise in humans. J Appl Physiol ; 78 : — Sahlin K, Tonkonogi M, Soderlund K.

Energy supply and muscle fatigue in humans. Acta Physiol Scand ; : — Chin ER, Allen DG. J Physiol ; : 17— Nielsen J, Schroder HD, Rix CG, Ortenblad N. Distinct effects of subcellular glycogen localization on tetanic relaxation time and endurance in mechanically skinned rat skeletal muscle fibres.

Ortenblad N, Nielsen J, Saltin B, Holmberg HC. Article PubMed CAS Google Scholar. Metzger JM, Moss RL. Effects of tension and stiffness due to reduced pH in mammalian fast- and slow-twitch skinned skeletal muscle fibres.

Pate E, Bhimani M, Franks-Skiba K, Cooke R. Reduced effect of pH on skinned rabbit psoas muscle mechanics at high temperatures: implications for fatigue. Stackhouse SK, Reisman DS, Binder-Macleod SA. Challenging the role of pH in skeletal muscle fatigue. Phys Ther ; 81 : — Westerblad H, Allen DG, Lannergren J.

Muscle fatigue: lactic acid or inorganic phosphate the major cause? News Physiol Sci ; 17 : 17— Allen DG, Trajanovska S. The multiple roles of phosphate in muscle fatigue.

Front Physiol ; 3 : Debold EP. Potential molecular mechanisms underlying muscle fatigue mediated by reactive oxygen and nitrogen species.

Front Physiol ; 6 : Delliaux S, Brerro-Saby C, Steinberg JG, Jammes Y. Reactive oxygen species activate the group IV muscle afferents in resting and exercising muscle in rats. Pflugers Arch ; : — Skeletal muscle fatigue: cellular mechanisms. Physiol Rev ; 88 : — Macdonald WA, Stephenson DG.

Effects of ADP on sarcoplasmic reticulum function in mechanically skinned skeletal muscle fibres of the rat. Panossian A, Wikman G. Evidence-based efficacy of adaptogens in fatigue, and molecular mechanisms related to their stress-protective activity.

Curr Clin Pharmacol ; 4 : — Huey KA, Meador BM. Contribution of IL-6 to the Hsp72, Hsp25, and alphaB-crystallin [corrected] responses to inflammation and exercise training in mouse skeletal and cardiac muscle.

Jammes Y, Steinberg JG, Olivier M, Brerro-Saby C, Condo J, Ravailhe S et al. The mechanisms of the widespread production of phosphorylated HSP25 after fatiguing muscle stimulation.

J Exp Biol ; : — Koh TJ. Do small heat shock proteins protect skeletal muscle from injury? Exerc Sport Sci Rev ; 30 : — Jiang B, Xiao W, Shi Y, Liu M, Xiao X.

Cell Stress Chaperones ; 10 : — Luo Z, Lei H, Sun Y, Liu X, Su DF. Orosomucoid, an acute response protein with multiple modulating activities. J Physiol Biochem ; 71 : — Lei H, Sun Y, Luo Z, Yourek G, Gui H, Yang Y et al.

Fatigue-induced Orosomucoid 1 acts on C-C chemokine receptor type 5 to enhance muscle endurance. Sci Rep ; 6 : Qin Z, Wan JJ, Sun Y, Wang PY, Su DF, Lei H et al.

ORM promotes skeletal muscle glycogen accumulation via CCR5-activated AMPK pathway in mice. Front Pharmacol ; 7 : Article PubMed PubMed Central CAS Google Scholar. Hoffman BW, Oya T, Carroll TJ, Cresswell AG. Increases in corticospinal responsiveness during a sustained submaximal plantar flexion.

McNeil CJ, Martin PG, Gandevia SC, Taylor JL. The response to paired motor cortical stimuli is abolished at a spinal level during human muscle fatigue. J Physiol-London ; 58 : — Levenez M, Garland SJ, Klass M, Duchateau J.

Cortical and spinal modulation of antagonist coactivation during a submaximal fatiguing contraction in humans. J Neurophysiol ; 99 : — Patikas DA, Bassa H, Kotzamanidis C. Changes in the reflex excitability during and after a sustained, low-intensity muscle contraction.

Int J Sports Med ; 27 : — Papaiordanidou M, Guiraud D, Varray A. Kinetics of neuromuscular changes during low-frequency electrical stimulation.

Muscle Nerve ; 41 : 54— Hwang IS, Huang CY, Wu PS, Chen YC, Wang CH. Assessment of H reflex sensitivity with M wave alternation consequent to fatiguing contractions. Int J Neurosci ; : — Kalmar JM, Del BC, Cafarelli E.

Increased spinal excitability does not offset central activation failure. Exp Brain Res ; : — Behm DG, St-Pierre DM. Effects of fatigue duration and muscle type on voluntary and evoked contractile properties. J Appl Physiol ; 82 : — Kent-Braun JA.

Central and peripheral contributions to muscle fatigue in humans during sustained maximal effort. Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol ; 80 : 57— Finsterer J.

Biomarkers of peripheral muscle fatigue during exercise. BMC Musculoskelet Disord ; 13 : Korzeniewski B. AMP deamination delays muscle acidification during heavy exercise and hypoxia. J Biol Chem ; : — Gosker HR, Schols AMWJ. Fatigued muscles in COPD but no finishing line in sight.

Eur Respir J ; 31 : — Malenfant S, Potus F, Fournier F, Breuils-Bonnet S, Pflieger A, Bourassa S et al. Skeletal muscle proteomic signature and metabolic impairment in pulmonary hypertension. J Mol Med ; 93 : — Ding Y, Fang HQ, Shang W, Xiao Y, Sun T, Hou N et al.

Mitoflash altered by metabolic stress in insulin-resistant skeletal muscle. Siegel AJ, Januzzi J, Sluss P, Lee-Lewandrowski E, Wood M, Shirey T et al. Cardiac biomarkers, electrolytes, and other analytes in collapsed marathon runners: implications for the evaluation of runners following competition.

Am J Clin Pathol ; : — Galal NM, Fouad HM, Saied A, Dabnon M. Hyperammonemia in the pediatric emergency care setting. Pediatr Emerg Care ; 26 : — Esbjornsson M, Norman B, Suchdev S, Viru M, Lindhgren A, Jansson E. Greater growth hormone and insulin response in women than in men during repeated bouts of sprint exercise.

Speranza L, Grilli A, Patruno A, Franceschelli S, Felzani G, Pesce M et al. Plasmatic markers of muscular stress in isokinetic exercise. J Biol Reg Homeos Ag ; 21 : 23— CAS Google Scholar. Puig JG, Mateos FA, Miranda ME, Torres RJ, de Miguel E, Perez de Ayala C et al. Purine metabolism in women with primary gout.

Am J Med ; 97 : — Bloomer RJ, Goldfarb AH, Wideman L, Mckenzie MJ, Consitt LA. Effects of acute aerobic and anaerobic exercise on blood markers of oxidative stress.

J Strength Cond Res ; 19 : — Margonis K, Fatouros IG, Jamurtas AZ, Nikolaidis MG, Douroudos L, Chatzinikolaou A et al. Oxidative stress biomarkers responses to physical overtraining: Implications for diagnosis. Free Radical Bio Med ; 43 : — Karsikas S, Myllymäki M, Heikkilä M, Sormunen R, Kivirikko KI, Myllyharju J et al.

HIF-P4H-2 deficiency protects against skeletal muscle ischemia-reperfusion injury. J Mol Med Berl ; 94 : — Robertson JD, Maughan RJ, Duthie GG, Morrice PC. Increased blood antioxidant systems of runners in response to training load. Clin Sci Lond ; 80 : — Ward WF, Qi W, Remmen HV, Zackert WE, Roberts LJ, Richardson A.

Effects of age and caloric restriction on lipid peroxidation: measurement of oxidative stress by F2-isoprostane levels. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci ; 60 : — Kawamoto EM, Vasconcelos AR, Degaspari S, Bohmer AE, Scavone C, Marcourakis T. Age-related changes in nitric oxide activity, cyclic GMP, and TBARS levels in platelets and erythrocytes reflect the oxidative status in central nervous system.

Age Dordr ; 35 : — Pandey KB, Mehdi MM, Maurya PK, Rizvi SI. Plasma protein oxidation and its correlation with antioxidant potential during human aging.

Dis Markers ; 29 : 31— Mendoza-Nunez VM, Ruiz-Ramos M, Sanchez-Rodriguez MA, Retana-Ugalde R, Munoz-Sanchez JL. Aging-related oxidative stress in healthy humans. Tohoku J Exp Med ; : — Cavalca V, Veglia F, Squellerio I, Marenzi G, Minardi F, De Metrio M et al.

Glutathione, vitamin E and oxidative stress in coronary artery disease: relevance of age and gender. Eur J Clin Invest ; 39 : — Vitai M, Goth L. Reference ranges of normal blood catalase activity and levels in familial hypocatalasemia in Hungary. Clin Chim Acta ; : 35— Jha R, Rizvi SI.

Carbonyl formation in erythrocyte membrane proteins during aging in humans. Biomed Pap Med Fac Univ Palacky Olomouc Czech Repub ; : 39— Li G, Liu L, Hu H, Zhao Q, Xie F, Chen K et al.

Age-related carbonyl stress and erythrocyte membrane protein carbonylation. Clin Hemorheol Microcirc ; 46 : — Palasuwan A, Suksom D, Margaritis I, Soogarun S, Rousseau AS.

Effects of tai chi training on antioxidant capacity in pre- and postmenopausal women. J Aging Res ; : Rosety-Rodriguez M, Rosety M, Ordonez FJ. Med Clin Barc ; : — Article Google Scholar. Tan TY, Lu CH, Lin TK, Liou CW, Chuang YC, Schminke U.

Factors associated with gender difference in the intima-media thickness of the common carotid artery. Clin Radiol ; 64 : — Basu S, Helmersson J, Jarosinska D, Sallsten G, Mazzolai B, Barregard L. Regulatory factors of basal F 2 -isoprostane formation: population, age, gender and smoking habits in humans.

Free Radic Res ; 43 : 85— Nanda N, Bobby Z, Hamide A. Oxidative stress and protein glycation in primary hypothyroidism. Clin Exp Med ; 8 : — Kamper EF, Chatzigeorgiou A, Tsimpoukidi O, Kamper M, Dalla C, Pitychoutis PM et al.

Physiol Behav ; 98 : — Simpson RJ, Florida-James GD, Whyte GP, Guy K. Eur J Appl Physiol ; 97 : — J Biol Regul Homeost Agents ; 21 : 21— Brandt C, Pedersen BK. The role of exercise-induced myokines in muscle homeostasis and the defense against chronic diseases.

J Biomed Biotechnol ; : Goetzl EJ, Huang MC, Kon J, Patel K, Schwartz JB, Fast K et al. Gender specificity of altered human immune cytokine profiles in aging.

FASEB J ; 24 : — Stulnig T, Maczek C, Böck G, Majdic O, Wick G. Reference intervals for human peripheral blood lymphocyte subpopulations from 'healthy' young and aged subjects. Int Arch Allergy Immunol ; : — Foster AD, Soloviova K, Puliaeva I, Puliaiev M, Puliaev R, Finkelman F et al. Donor CD8 T cells and IFN-gamma are critical for sex-based differences in donor CD4 T cell engraftment and lupus-like phenotype in short-term chronic graft-versus-host disease mice.

J Immunol ; : — Nijs J, Van Oosterwijck J, Meeus M, Lambrecht L, Metzger K, Fremont M et al. J Intern Med ; : — Nielsen AR, Mounier R, Plomgaard P, Mortensen OH, Penkowa M, Speerschneider T et al.

Expression of interleukin in human skeletal muscle effect of exercise and muscle fibre type composition. The J Physioly ; : — Finsterer J, Milvay E. Stress lactate in mitochondrial myopathy under constant, unadjusted workload.

Eur J Neurol ; 11 : — Weiss B, Laties VG. Enhancement of human performance by caffeine and the amphetamines. Pharmacol Rev ; 14 : 1— Wyndham CH, Rogers GG, Benade AJ, Strydom NB. Physiological effects of the amphetamines during exercise. S Afr Med J ; 45 : — Gerald MC.

Neuropharmacology ; 17 : — Morozova E, Yoo Y, Behrouzvaziri A, Zaretskaia M, Rusyniak D, Zaretsky D et al. Stress due to employment or study pressure is another known precursor of fatigue, as is the temptation to continue training or playing sport while carrying an existing injury.

Most episodes of fatigue can be mitigated by first identifying the cause, followed by adjustments to training or activity schedules. The symptoms of fatigue will usually disappear quickly, however, in cases where fatigue symptoms persist a medical review will be necessary.

Anaemia and iron deficiency are common causes of fatigue, as are some infections. Fatigue is a symptom of major illnesses including heart disease, diabetes, sleep apnoea and thyroid problems.

A visit to your Doctor or Sports and Exercise Medicine Physician will help rule out these possibilities while honing in on the actual cause of fatigue. Muscle fatigue takes place when there is a decrease in power of the muscle, and is experienced in different ways according to the activity performed.

Fatigue can set in when muscle tension cannot be maintained. They will often be sore days later as a result of acute muscle breakdown. Interval training helps by conditioning muscle fibres to respond more adequately for longer periods.

Here, we performed a meta-analysis to investigate these inconsistent findings. However, a three-parameter selection model also revealed evidence of publication or reporting biases, suggesting that the bias-corrected estimates might be substantially lower 0.

In sum, current evidence does not provide conclusive support for the claim that mental fatigue has a negative influence on exercise performance. Review Article. Mental Fatigue Might Be Not So Bad for Exercise Performance After All: A Systematic Review and Bias-Sensitive Meta-Analysis.

Authors: Darías Holgado Email Darías Holgado.

Exercise and fatigue

Although self-regulation and self-control are often used interchangeably [ ], it was proposed that they refer to distinct processes [ 64 ]. While self-regulation refers to more general processes of goal-directed thoughts and behaviors, self-control can be defined as the process of overcoming predominant pre-potent, automatic response tendencies in favor of the desired goal [ 65 ].

Self-control is exerted during the execution of sustained cognitive tasks and requires motivation as well as attention [ 64 ]. This is in line with the view that performing a sustained cognitive task competes with motivational options e.

In addition to these key-determinants, there are further important aspects that contribute to the psychophysiological state of an individual with potential consequences for perceived cognitive fatigue. For instance, it has been revealed that greater interest in a cognitive task resulted in less perceived cognitive fatigue despite a higher willingness to exert cognitive effort [ ].

In the same manner, it was argued that the level of controllability modulates the aversive responses and perceived cognitive fatigue during sustained cognitive activities [ ]. Furthermore, the execution of sustained cognitive tasks can be associated with changes in mood as well as with feelings like stress, anxiety, frustration, hopelessness, tension, and boredom [ , , , ].

Some of these were shown to be related to cognitive task performance [ ] and to modulate perceived cognitive fatigue [ 64 , , ]. For example, it was found that a task requiring the passive observation of strings of numbers resulted in higher boredom ratings, a steeper decline in affective valence, and higher perceived cognitive fatigue ratings compared to a cognitive task that consisted of adding three to each digit of a four-digit number [ ].

Interestingly, passively watching strings of numbers was also rated as effortful, which was interpreted as the effort to keep paying attention. It was also proposed that boredom, caused by the low intrinsic attractiveness of the task itself, can also be responsible for a decrease in cognitive task performance [ ], highlighting the interdependence between determinants of perceived cognitive fatigue and cognitive performance fatigue.

Moreover, it was assumed that expectations based on previous experiences might influence the psychophysiological state and the psychophysiological adjustments during sustained cognitive tasks [ ]. For instance, this was shown in response to heat stress [ , ], sleep deprivation [ ], and mouth rinsing with caffeine-maltodextrin [ ].

These sources of influence also highlight the similarity between the constructs of sleepiness and fatigue. Confusion between those concepts is very common and it remains unclear to what extent the subjective assessment measures allow researchers to clearly tease them apart [ ]. Besides these, there are further factors contributing to perceived cognitive fatigue that are increasingly studied and should be added to the list of potential determinants in the future.

Various modulating factors can influence the extent of cognitive performance fatigue and perceived cognitive fatigue Fig. The main subject-specific factors include age, sex, the existence of diseases, and cognitive fitness.

The extent of fatigue in the different domains is further determined by the characteristics of the cognitive task e. In the following sections, we will only discuss the most important subject- and cognitive task-specific factors that can modulate cognitive performance fatigue and perceived cognitive fatigue.

Cognitive abilities, such as processing speed and executive functioning, decline with advancing age due to structural and functional changes within the brain e. It is thought that these age-related changes contribute to the increased compensatory brain activation observed during the execution of cognitive tasks [ ], which was assumed to accelerate cognitive performance fatigue development [ ].

However, the results of studies that have investigated the effect of age on cognitive performance fatigue are mixed and do not allow for a definite conclusion. For instance, it was found that reaction times remained constant in young and old adults after the execution of a working memory task performed for 60 min, while accuracy even increased in the elderly.

The authors proposed that the potential decline in cognitive performance was countered by the learning effect [ ]. This is in line with the results of Behrens et al. In contrast, Terentjeviene et al. However, reaction times and intra-individual variability of reaction times only increased in young males indicating higher cognitive performance fatigue compared to the older males.

Since brain activation differs between sexes depending on the type of cognitive task [ , ], it might be assumed that the extent of cognitive performance fatigue after a sustained cognitive activity is different between males and females. However, the results of experiments on that topic are inconclusive.

For instance, there were no sex-differences in the error rate and reaction times, which remained constant over time, after performing a continuous performance test for 51 min [ ].

This is consistent with the results of Wang et al. The authors did not observe differences in the number of errors and completed subtractions between males and females.

However, the task lasted only 12 min and was possibly not long enough to induce cognitive performance fatigue.

In contrast to these studies, Noreika et al. They have revealed that accuracy increased, and response times decreased to a larger extent in males compared to females with time-on-task.

However, since cognitive performance increased over time, these results did not indicate cognitive performance fatigue and might be biased by the learning or practice effect. Since the extent of cognitive performance fatigue strongly depends on the structural and functional integrity of the central nervous system, it might be assumed that cognitive performance fatigue development is accelerated in patient populations, especially in those with neurological diseases affecting the central nervous system.

However, studies investigating cognitive performance fatigue in different patient populations e. Despite similar increases in cognitive task performance, patients showed heightened cerebral activation in specific areas and an increased perceived cognitive fatigue [ 10 , , ] indicating a lower efficiency.

In contrast, a decrease in cognitive performance measures was revealed during the execution of a four-block paced auditory serial addition test with an earlier drop in performance in people with multiple sclerosis compared to healthy controls [ ].

Analogous results were obtained by other studies that investigated the effects of sustained cognitive tasks on cognitive performance fatigue in multiple sclerosis and healthy controls [ , ].

A decrease in cognitive task performance was also found in stroke survivors while performing a min inhibition task, which was, however, comparable to the cognitive task performance reduction of the healthy control group [ ]. Moreover, Jordan et al.

The discrepant findings presented above indicate that the effect of age, sex, and diseases on cognitive performance fatigue seems to be strongly influenced by the task characteristics e. The occurrence and evaluation of cognitive performance fatigue strongly depend on the type of task e.

For instance, Smith et al. While the first task required only vigilance, the other two tasks additionally relied on response inhibition. Cognitive performance i. Interestingly, cognitive performance monitored during the respective tasks deteriorated only for the psychomotor vigilance task i.

In contrast, pre- and post-cognitive performance assessments with the 3-min psychomotor vigilance task revealed increased reaction times only after the AX-continuous performance task and the Stroop task. These data indicate that the detection and extent of cognitive performance fatigue strongly depend on the type of task as well as the task used to assess the potential change in performance.

Besides the effect of the type of cognitive task, other task characteristics e. Although several studies have not found a decline in cognitive performance with time-on-task, it was demonstrated that cognitive task performance decreased with increasing task duration [ 21 , , ].

Furthermore, it was proposed that the cognitive load induced by the task determines the extent of cognitive performance fatigue [ , , ]. Studies on this topic manipulated the cognitive load either by increasing the difficulty of the task e.

However, the results of these studies are inconsistent. For instance, during a min working memory task, Shigihara et al. The authors have additionally tested the effect of the sustained cognitive tasks on the advanced trail making test performance and have found that the number of errors increased from pre to post for both conditions.

Moreover, the results of Borragàn et al. However, irrespective of the task characteristics, there is a general critique that laboratory cognitive tasks are not ecologically valid and exhibit low intrinsic motivational value.

Therefore, controllability of these tasks is low, people experience them as increasingly aversive, and might disengage from the tasks because they are not sufficiently important for them [ 22 , 64 , , ].

Subject-specific factors like age, sex, and the presence of diseases might also modulate perceived cognitive fatigue and its potential determinants. For example, Wascher et al. Interestingly, young adults showed also a concomitant larger decrease in self-reported motivation.

This is in line with the results of Terentjeviene et al. This was accompanied by a higher perceived cognitive effort and temporal demand in the young participants during task execution. The young adults additionally reported increases in tension and confusion as well as a decrease in vigor, which were not found for the older participants.

These data collectively suggest that younger adults perceive sustained cognitive tasks as more effortful, demanding, and fatiguing than older adults, which is corroborated by the larger decreases in motivation and vigor as well as the increased tension and confusion.

Since laboratory cognitive tasks are often performed using a computer and older adults have less experience with digital technology [ ], these differences might be related to the higher intrinsic attractiveness of the task for older people.

Nevertheless, it was also shown that older people with a low frequency of technology use have higher levels of computer anxiety [ ]. Contrary to the findings of age-related differences, it has also been observed that the increase in perceived cognitive fatigue induced by a min inhibitory control task was not different between young and old adults [ 11 ].

Studies on sex-differences in perceived cognitive fatigue also revealed partially inconsistent results. Some studies have not found differences in perceived cognitive fatigue between males and females after performing a min Stroop task and a min continuous performance test, which require response inhibition and sustained attention.

Similarly, no sex-differences in the changes in cognitive effort, vigor, energy, tiredness, tension, calmness, and further self-reported data recorded during these tasks were reported [ , ].

Nevertheless, others have observed higher increases in fatigue ratings during a min mental rotation task in females compared to males, which depended on the menstrual cycle phase [ ]. It was additionally revealed that perceived cognitive effort and perceived task difficulty were higher in females compared to males during both a high and low cognitive load condition involving arithmetic tasks [ ].

There is evidence that perceived cognitive fatigue development in response to sustained cognitive tasks is higher in some diseases, especially those affecting the central nervous system [ 10 , , , ].

Higher increases in fatigue ratings during cognitive tasks have particularly been observed in people with multiple sclerosis [ , , ]. It was further reported that the perceived workload e. In contrast, studies on other patient populations, such as people with chronic fatigue syndrome, depression, and myasthenia gravis, have not found differential changes in perceived cognitive fatigue ratings compared with healthy controls during sustained cognitive tasks [ , ].

It has been shown that perceived cognitive fatigue and its determinants can be influenced by the type of task e. The results of studies that have examined the effect of the type of cognitive task on perceived cognitive fatigue are inconclusive. The latter was applied with constant processing intervals and with shorter individualized processing intervals based on the maximal performance determined during an incremental TloadDback task.

Although the task duration differed greatly, all cognitive tasks induced perceived cognitive fatigue, assessed with a visual analog scale, with the highest increase in the individualized TloadDback task condition. However, the AX continuous performance test induced a higher increase in perceived cognitive fatigue and a larger decrease in vigor assessed with the Brunel Mood Scale.

These results were accompanied by higher sleepiness ratings and a larger drop in task motivation compared to the TloadDback task condition. Another study on this topic has compared perceived cognitive fatigue as well as its recovery in response to a psychomotor vigilance task, an AX-continuous performance task, and a Stroop task each performed for 45 min [ ].

The tasks differed regarding their demands on vigilance and response inhibition. Although all tasks increased perceived cognitive fatigue, the authors concluded that tasks requiring response inhibition appeared to induce perceived cognitive fatigue for a longer duration than a simple vigilance task.

With regard to other task characteristics, it was often found that perceived cognitive fatigue progressively increases with time-on-task [ , ]. Moreover, it was shown that altering the cognitive load by the difficulty of the task i.

In line with this, Borragàn et al. However, when cognitive load was modulated by decreasing the processing interval for the stimuli, the high cognitive load condition induced a higher increase in perceived cognitive fatigue compared to the low cognitive load condition. The authors concluded that the processing time interval during cognitive tasks is more relevant for perceived cognitive fatigue development than the number of processed items.

In contrast to this, it was also shown that perceived cognitive fatigue cannot only result from performing a sustained cognitive task i. More specifically, perceived cognitive fatigue was even higher in the boredom condition compared with the cognitive task condition despite lower cognitive effort ratings.

This might be related to the steeper decline in affective valence ratings and the lower task interest ratings in the boredom condition.

Indeed, higher interest in a task has been shown to induce less perceived cognitive fatigue [ ]. These data highlight the importance of the characteristics of the cognitive task for the development of perceived cognitive fatigue and its determinants. The updated framework covers the different dimensions of task-induced state fatigue and the involved mechanisms Fig.

Thereby, the interdependence of performance fatigue and perceived fatigue as well as their determinants is acknowledged and highlighted. There is no single factor primarily determining performance fatigue and perceived fatigue in response to motor and cognitive tasks. Instead, the relative weight of each determinant and their interaction depends on several modulating factors e.

Although the mechanisms of motor performance fatigue are not yet fully elucidated, there are extensive data on the changes in the nervous system and muscle during motor tasks contributing to the decline in motor performance [ 16 , 17 , 25 , 26 ].

In contrast, the mechanisms underlying perceived motor fatigue and their interactions with motor performance fatigue received less attention. Therefore, future research should not only investigate the neural and muscular mechanisms driving motor performance fatigue but also aspects of perceived motor fatigue and the corresponding neuro physiological correlates in detail.

This approach can assist in investigating the motor task-induced perceptual differences between individuals and exercise protocols e. This is of special importance for clinical populations suffering from an increased prevalence of motor performance fatigue and perceived motor fatigue e.

There are a few studies available that have assessed neural and muscular contributions to motor performance fatigue in parallel with ratings of perceived motor fatigue, effort perception, and affective valence to study their interactions [ 42 , ].

For instance, Greenhouse-Tucknott et al. They have shown that prior submaximal hand grip exercise reduced the time to exhaustion during a submaximal isometric contraction of the knee extensors without altering neuromuscular function.

Thereby, effort perception and affective valence were correlated with time to exhaustion and the ratings of perceived motor fatigue. These findings indicate that prior handgrip exercise limited single-joint endurance performance of the knee extensors primarily by the interactions between perceived motor fatigue, effort perception, as well as affective valence and not by a decreased neuromuscular function.

Similar approaches should be adopted in the future to investigate the interactions between motor performance fatigue and perceived motor fatigue in different populations, particularly in those suffering from diseases. Besides the combined investigation of motor performance fatigue, perceived motor fatigue, and the underlying mechanisms, the determining factors can be manipulated to elucidate their causal involvement in the development of state fatigue in different populations and in response to various motor tasks.

For that purpose, different interventions can be used aiming to modify the physiological and psychological regulatory processes during fatiguing motor exercise.

For instance, neuromodulation techniques like tDCS are suitable to alter cortical excitability and to investigate the effects of changed neural properties on motor performance fatigue and perceived motor fatigue [ 67 ].

Furthermore, other interventions can be applied to modify neural as well as muscular properties e. Interventions aiming to modulate the psychological determinants of endurance performance have also been shown to induce changes in motor performance and the perceptual responses to fatiguing exercise [ 48 ].

These strategies could be used to investigate the role of cognitive processes in the interpretation of perceptual responses and the change in affective valence emerging during fatiguing motor exercise.

Motor performance fatigue can be assessed using maximal and submaximal motor performance measures. Maximal performance tasks e. In addition, the variation of submaximal motor performance is also an indication of motor performance fatigue e.

The neural and muscular mechanisms contributing to motor performance fatigue can be investigated with different non-invasive techniques. Neural adjustments i. Moreover, functional magnetic resonance imaging is suitable to monitor changes within cortical and subcortical structures during motor exercise [ ].

The contractile function of muscles can be validly quantified using peripheral nerve stimulation [ ], while changes in muscle oxygenation and muscle metabolism can be measured with near-infrared spectroscopy and phosphorus magnetic resonance spectroscopy, respectively [ , ].

In addition to these measures, perceived motor fatigue as well as the contributing factors should be comprehensively assessed before, during, and after fatiguing exercise. For this purpose, different scales and questionnaires can be used according to the focus of the respective study.

These measures should be applied together with standardized wording as described elsewhere [ 5 , 38 ]. Furthermore, the attentional focus during fatiguing motor exercise should be recorded as an index for the motor task intensity-dependent attentional shift from an external focus on the surrounding to an internal focus on the bodily sensations [ ].

These aspects should be quantified in conjunction with affective valence and arousal, recorded with the feeling scale and felt arousal scale, respectively, as indicators of the motor task-dependent homeostatic perturbations [ 40 , 60 ].

It has been shown that these aspects can influence perceived motor fatigue and performance during fatiguing motor tasks. Moreover, they reflect the motor task-induced homeostatic perturbations in various physiological subsystems and are thus indicators of the physical demands.

Besides these core measures of perceived motor fatigue, additional scales and tests should be used according to the aim of the respective study. This should be done to investigate the role of self-regulation capacity, executive functioning [ 41 , , ], and other determinants for motor performance fatigue as well as perceived motor fatigue.

The interactions between cognitive performance fatigue and perceived cognitive fatigue have been investigated more comprehensively and in greater detail compared to those between motor performance fatigue and perceived motor fatigue.

Accordingly, many studies in this field have recorded both changes in cognitive performance as well as in the perception of fatigue during and after sustained cognitive tasks. However, it must be pointed out that in studies which have measured cognitive performance on the same task as the one used to induce fatigue, evidence for a decline in cognitive task performance was frequently missing [ , ].

The lack of a systematic decline in cognitive performance with time-on-task was often attributed to an increased compensatory cognitive effort or to a learning effect that would lead to a performance increase overcoming the performance decline induced by fatigue [ , , , ].

In contrast, increases in perceived cognitive fatigue with time-on-task have been shown very consistently across many different conditions e. Therefore, future studies should use separate cognitive tasks to induce and measure cognitive performance fatigue as already done by some studies [ 23 , 24 , , , ].

This approach might bypass the influence of a decreased motivation or learning effect and has typically led to more consistent correlations between the cognitive performance decline and perceived cognitive fatigue [ 24 , ].

Furthermore, the effect of distinct types e. There is evidence that different cognitive tasks induce specific declines in performance measures depending on the assessment task. On the contrary, pre and post cognitive performance assessments with a 3-min psychomotor vigilance task revealed only increased reaction times after the AX-continuous performance task and the Stroop task.

However, perceived cognitive fatigue increased in all conditions, even though it tended to persist longer after the tasks requiring more response inhibition. Moreover, cognitive performance fatigue and perceived cognitive fatigue measures were also shown to be sensitive to the manipulation of the cognitive load [ , ].

Consequently, future studies on this topic should not only examine the effects of diverse types of tasks on cognitive performance fatigue, perceived cognitive fatigue, and their neural correlates but also the influence of varying cognitive loads.

Furthermore, it is likely that the level of overlap between the fatiguing and the assessment tasks, in terms of the cognitive processes involved, is also crucial [ ].

Therefore, it appears essential that future studies assess performance before and after the fatiguing cognitive task not only with cognitive tasks requiring similar cognitive processes, but also with tasks that involve different processes. These experiments should further take the impact of the cognitive load, the nature of the cognitive processes involved, and the degree of process overlap with the fatiguing task into account.

As stated above, other important sources of influence are mood and emotional variables, like stress, anxiety, frustration, hopelessness, tension, and boredom [ , , , ], which were shown to modulate perceived cognitive fatigue [ , , ] and cognitive task performance [ ].

Therefore, it seems mandatory to quantify these aspects and to analyze their effects on cognitive performance fatigue, perceived cognitive fatigue, and their neural correlates. Due to the discrepant findings, the influence of subject-specific factors i.

Nevertheless, it seems that cognitive task-induced perceived cognitive fatigue is higher in some patient populations [ , , ]. To address the causal relationships, neurophysiological and psychophysiological determinants of cognitive performance fatigue as well as perceived cognitive fatigue can also be modulated experimentally.

For instance, it was shown that anodal tDCS applied to the right parietal cortex counteracted the cognitive performance decline during a min visual vigilance task in healthy controls and people with multiple sclerosis but had no effect on perceived cognitive fatigue [ ].

Similar effects were observed in multiple sclerosis patients after stimulating the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex with anodal tDCS [ ]. In addition, investigating the effects of different neuromodulatory substances such as caffeine on cognitive performance fatigue, perceived cognitive fatigue [ ], and its neurophysiological correlates can help to unravel their interdependence.

As outlined above, the detection and quantification of cognitive performance fatigue strongly depend on the assessment task and the considered variables e.

These measures should be combined with techniques suitable to record brain activity e. Ample evidence points to the interactions between the different dimensions of activity-induced state fatigue.

For instance, Marcora et al. However, effort perception during exercise was higher after performing the sustained cognitive task leading the authors to the conclusion that the participants reached their maximal tolerable effort level earlier and subsequently disengaged from exercise.

These data indicate that fatiguing cognitive or motor activities seem to modulate the performance and perceptions during a subsequent fatiguing motor or cognitive task, respectively. Evidence for the interactions between the different dimensions of activity-induced state fatigue also arises from experiments that have investigated the psychophysiological adjustments and performance changes in response to sustained motor-cognitive dual tasks [ , , ].

These studies have found a decreased time to exhaustion during submaximal motor tasks when a concurrent cognitive task had to be executed e.

Additionally, time to exhaustion during a fatiguing motor-cognitive dual task tended to be shorter for a high compared to a low cognitive load condition. This was accompanied by a higher reduction in muscle activation of the knee extensors i. As expected, cognitive effort perception scaled with the level of cognitive load, but, surprisingly, effort perception associated with the motor task was greater in the high cognitive load condition compared to that recorded during the single motor task condition [ ].

These data have impressively shown that the different domains of activity-induced state fatigue interact with each other. Since there is an overlap of brain structures involved during the execution of fatiguing motor and cognitive tasks e. Thereby, the degree of overlap between cognitive processes required for the respective motor and cognitive task might mediate the detrimental effects on performance and perceptions [ 9 , , ].

Consequently, future studies should investigate the effects of diverse types e. These effects should also be studied in relation to various motor tasks e.

Conversely, the impact of different fatiguing motor tasks performed prior to various fatiguing cognitive tasks relying on distinct cognitive processes should be examined.

The mechanistic basis for the interactions between motor performance fatigue, perceived motor fatigue, cognitive performance fatigue, and perceived cognitive fatigue can be probed with the methods mentioned in the respective paragraphs above.

However, the quantification of some perceptual responses to motor and cognitive tasks requires specific scales and a clear description with standardized wording. This is of particular importance for the measurement of perceived fatigue and effort in response to motor, cognitive, or motor-cognitive dual tasks.

Although it was proposed that the feeling of fatigue arising from exertion might be similarly induced by motor and cognitive tasks [ 9 ], results of studies indicated that perceived motor fatigue and perceived cognitive fatigue represent different perceptual domains [ , ].

The same applies to effort perception, which can be related to either to the motor or the cognitive task [ ].

Performance fatigue and perceived fatigue as well as their determinants are interdependent and should not be considered in isolation. Consequently, there is no single factor primarily determining performance fatigue and perceived fatigue in response to motor and cognitive tasks.

Instead, the relative weight of each determinant and their interaction depend on body homeostasis e. Therefore, a combined assessment of performance fatigue and perceived fatigue measures as well as its neuro physiological correlates is required to unravel the psychophysiology of motor and cognitive task-induced state fatigue.

This will help to better understand the interactions between the different dimensions of fatigue and their impact on human performance, which is necessary to design effective interventions for increasing exercise tolerance and human performance in healthy and clinical populations.

Enoka RM, Duchateau J. Muscle fatigue: what, why and how it influences muscle function. J Physiol. Article CAS Google Scholar. Kluger BM, Krupp LB, Enoka RM. Fatigue and fatigability in neurologic illnesses: proposal for a unified taxonomy. Article Google Scholar. Translating fatigue to human performance.

Med Sci Sports Exerc. Tommasin S, de Luca F, Ferrante I, Gurreri F, Castelli L, Ruggieri S, et al. Cognitive fatigability is a quantifiable distinct phenomenon in multiple sclerosis. J Neuropsychol. Venhorst A, Micklewright D, Noakes TD. Perceived fatigability: utility of a three-dimensional dynamical systems framework to better understand the psychophysiological regulation of goal-directed exercise behaviour.

Sports Med. Braley TJ, Chervin RD. Fatigue in multiple sclerosis: mechanisms, evaluation, and treatment. Gruet M. Fatigue in chronic respiratory diseases: theoretical framework and implications for real-life performance and rehabilitation.

Front Physiol. Marrelli K, Cheng AJ, Brophy JD, Power GA. Perceived versus performance fatigability in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Müller T, Apps MAJ. Motivational fatigue: a neurocognitive framework for the impact of effortful exertion on subsequent motivation.

Genova HM, Rajagopalan V, Deluca J, Das A, Binder A, Arjunan A, et al. Examination of cognitive fatigue in multiple sclerosis using functional magnetic resonance imaging and diffusion tensor imaging.

PLoS ONE. Behrens M, Mau-Moeller A, Lischke A, Katlun F, Gube M, Zschorlich V, et al. Mental fatigue increases gait variability during dual-task walking in old adults.

J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. Behrens M, Broscheid K-C, Schega L. Taxonomie und Determinanten motorischer performance fatigability bei Multipler Sklerose. Micklewright D, St Clair Gibson A, Gladwell V, Al Salman A. Development and validity of the rating-of-fatigue scale. Gandevia SC.

Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol Rev. Noakes TD. Fatigue is a brain-derived emotion that regulates the exercise behavior to ensure the protection of whole-body homeostasis.

Taylor JL, Amann M, Duchateau J, Meeusen R, Rice CL. Neural contributions to muscle fatigue: from the brain to the muscle and back again. Taylor JL, Gandevia SC. A comparison of central aspects of fatigue in submaximal and maximal voluntary contractions.

J Appl Physiol. Marcora SM, Staiano W, Manning V. Mental fatigue impairs physical performance in humans. Blain GM, Mangum TS, Sidhu SK, Weavil JC, Hureau TJ, Jessop JE, et al. Laurin J, Pertici V, Dousset E, Marqueste T, Decherchi P. Group III and IV muscle afferents: role on central motor drive and clinical implications.

Boksem MAS, Tops M. Mental fatigue: costs and benefits. Brain Res Rev. Kurzban R. The sense of effort. Curr Opin Psychol. Benoit C-E, Solopchuk O, Borragán G, Carbonnelle A, van Durme S, Zénon A. Cognitive task avoidance correlates with fatigue-induced performance decrement but not with subjective fatigue.

Gergelyfi M, Sanz-Arigita EJ, Solopchuk O, Dricot L, Jacob B, Zénon A. Mental fatigue correlates with depression of task-related network and augmented DMN activity but spares the reward circuit. Allen DG, Lamb GD, Westerblad H.

Skeletal muscle fatigue: cellular mechanisms. Cheng AJ, Place N, Westerblad H. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med. Ebenbichler GR, Kollmitzer J, Glckler L, Bochdansky T, Kopf A, Fialka V.

The role of the biarticular agonist and cocontracting antagonist pair in isometric muscle fatigue. Muscle Nerve. Gagnon D, Bertrand Arsenault A, Smyth G, Kemp F. Cocontraction changes in muscular fatigue at different levels of isometric contraction.

Int J Ind Ergon. Allen DG, Clugston E, Petersen Y, Röder IV, Chapman B, Rudolf R. Interactions between intracellular calcium and phosphate in intact mouse muscle during fatigue. Westerblad H.

Acidosis is not a significant cause of skeletal muscle fatigue. Hunter SK. Performance fatigability: mechanisms and task specificity. Nybo L, Rasmussen P, Sawka MN. Performance in the heat-physiological factors of importance for hyperthermia-induced fatigue.

Compr Physiol. Goodall S, González-Alonso J, Ali L, Ross EZ, Romer LM. Supraspinal fatigue after normoxic and hypoxic exercise in humans. Nybo L. CNS fatigue and prolonged exercise: effect of glucose supplementation.

Vargas NT, Marino F. A neuroinflammatory model for acute fatigue during exercise. Skau S, Sundberg K, Kuhn H-G. A proposal for a unifying set of definitions of fatigue. Front Psychol. Jones AM, Burnley M, Black MI, Poole DC, Vanhatalo A.

Pageaux B. Perception of effort in exercise science: definition, measurement and perspectives. Eur J Sport Sci. Mauger AR. Fatigue is a pain-the use of novel neurophysiological techniques to understand the fatigue—pain relationship. Ekkekakis P, Hall EE, Petruzzello SJ. Variation and homogeneity in affective responses to physical activity of varying intensities: an alternative perspective on dose-response based on evolutionary considerations.

J Sports Sci. Hyland-Monks R, Cronin L, McNaughton L, Marchant D. The role of executive function in the self-regulation of endurance performance: a critical review.

Prog Brain Res. Greenhouse-Tucknott A, Wrightson JG, Raynsford M, Harrison NA, Dekerle J. Interactions between perceptions of fatigue, effort, and affect decrease knee extensor endurance performance following upper body motor activity, independent of changes in neuromuscular function.

Marcora SM. Do we really need a central governor to explain brain regulation of exercise performance? Eur J Appl Physiol. Staiano W, Bosio A, de Morree HM, Rampinini E, Marcora S. The cardinal exercise stopper: muscle fatigue, muscle pain or perception of effort?

Marcora S. Perception of effort during exercise is independent of afferent feedback from skeletal muscles, heart, and lungs. Marcora SM, Staiano W. The limit to exercise tolerance in humans: mind over muscle?

Doherty M, Smith PM. Effects of caffeine ingestion on rating of perceived exertion during and after exercise: a meta-analysis.

Scand J Med Sci Sports. McCormick A, Meijen C, Marcora S. Psychological determinants of whole-body endurance performance. Terry PC, Karageorghis CI, Curran ML, Martin OV, Parsons-Smith RL. Effects of music in exercise and sport: a meta-analytic review.

Psychol Bull. Smirmaul BPC, de Moraes AC, Angius L, Marcora SM. Effects of caffeine on neuromuscular fatigue and performance during high-intensity cycling exercise in moderate hypoxia. Husmann F, Bruhn S, Mittlmeier T, Zschorlich V, Behrens M.

Dietary nitrate supplementation improves exercise tolerance by reducing muscle fatigue and perceptual responses. Behrens M, Zschorlich V, Mittlmeier T, Bruhn S, Husmann F. Ischemic preconditioning did not affect central and peripheral factors of performance fatigability after submaximal isometric exercise.

Nybo L, Nielsen B. Hyperthermia and central fatigue during prolonged exercise in humans. Barley OR, Chapman DW, Blazevich AJ, Abbiss CR.

Acute dehydration impairs endurance without modulating neuromuscular function. Romer LM, Haverkamp HC, Amann M, Lovering AT, Pegelow DF, Dempsey JA. Effect of acute severe hypoxia on peripheral fatigue and endurance capacity in healthy humans.

Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol. Temesi J, Arnal PJ, Davranche K, Bonnefoy R, Levy P, Verges S, Millet GY. Does central fatigue explain reduced cycling after complete sleep deprivation?

Astokorki AHY, Mauger AR. Transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation reduces exercise-induced perceived pain and improves endurance exercise performance. Smith SA, Micklewright D, Winter SL, Mauger AR.

Muscle pain induced by hypertonic saline in the knee extensors decreases single-limb isometric time to task failure. Hartman ME, Ekkekakis P, Dicks ND, Pettitt RW. Dynamics of pleasure-displeasure at the limit of exercise tolerance: conceptualizing the sense of exertional physical fatigue as an affective response.

J Exp Biol. Ekkekakis P, Zenko Z. Measurement of affective responses to exercise. In: Emotion measurement. Amsterdam: Elsevier; HSPs are involved in the adaptation to fatigue stress.

Within the family of HSPs, HSP25 protein is abundantly expressed in skeletal muscle and increases with muscle contractile activity. have reported that a widespread HSP25 response to fatigue in a single hindlimb muscle is responsible for a global adaptive response to acute localized stress and have demonstrated that group III and IV muscle afferents play an important role in the fatigue-induced p-HSP25 response; moreover, the sympathetic nerve supply to the muscles and kidney comprises the efferent arm of the p-HSP25 activation.

Orosomucoid ORM is an acute-phase protein, with a very low pI of 2. It is predominantly synthesized in the liver, and many extra-hepatic tissues have also been reported to produce ORM under physiological and pathological stress. Interestingly, exogenous ORM increases muscle glycogen and enhances muscle endurance, whereas ORM deficiency results in decreased muscle endurance, thus indicating that ORM is an endogenous anti-fatigue protein.

Further studies have demonstrated that ORM binds to C—C chemokine receptor type 5 CCR5 on muscle cells and activates AMPK, thus promoting glycogen storage and enhancing muscle endurance, and representing a positive feedback mechanism for resisting fatigue and maintaining homeostasis.

Muscle fatigue is manifested most naturally in the intact organism. Non-invasive techniques of site-specific stimulation can now be used to evaluate the potential sites of the entire system for force production in human studies.

All evoked muscle responses are recorded via electromyography EMG electrodes placed on the muscle. Transcranial magnetic stimulation involves applying magnetic stimulation to the motor cortex and is optimized to activate the muscle of interest.

MEP is influenced not only by cortical excitability but also by spinal cord motor neuron excitability and muscle factors.

MEP depression can occur in the relaxed muscle after a fatiguing exercise, possibly as a result of afferent input from the fatigued muscle. MEP is increased in the upper- and lower-limb muscles during sustained submaximal isometric contractions and is regarded as an augmentation of the central drive to the lower motoneuron pool that allows a constant level of force to be maintained despite the development of peripheral fatigue.

During sustained MVC, MEP has been reported to increase during the first seconds and then to level off, increase linearly or remain stable, depending on the protocol used that is, continuous vs intermittent and the muscle investigated.

Electrical stimulation in the cervicomedullary region aims to activate the corticospinal tract at a subcortical level, thereby eliminating cortical contributions to the evoked muscle response. The muscular response recorded by EMG is known as the cervicomedullary motor-evoked potential CMEP.

Comparison of MEP and CMEP is helpful for the localization of excitability at the cortical or subcortical level. Low-intensity electrical stimulation of the peripheral nerve preferentially activates the Ia sensory fibers, which synapse with the α-motoneuron in the spinal cord. The signal is then carried along the motor neurons to the muscle, generating a response in the muscle known as the Hoffmann reflex H-reflex.

The H-reflex is used to assess spinal excitability and inhibition. Although there are several of an increase 65 or no change, 66 the general consensus is that there is an overall decline in the amplitude of the H-reflex with the development of muscle fatigue, thus indicating a decrease in spinal excitability.

High-intensity stimulation of the peripheral nerve directly activates the α-motoneuron, evoking a motor response m-wave from the muscle.

The m-wave is a compound action potential recorded with surface EMG and is used to assess peripheral excitability of the muscle membrane and transmission at the neuromuscular junction.

A change in the twitch force without a change in the m-wave indicates a failure of excitation-contraction coupling. At present, there are still no specific factors that have been consistently associated with a particular type of fatigue. According to the mechanism and metabolic changes during muscle fatigue, three categories of biomarkers have been determined: 1 ATP metabolism biomarkers, such as lactate, ammonia and hypoxanthine; 2 Oxidative stress biomarkers ROS , such as lipid peroxidation, protein peroxidation, and antioxidative capacity; and 3 Inflammatory biomarkers, such as TNF-α, leukocytes, and interleukins.

When the ATP supply fails to meet the consumption of ATP during exercise, fatigue occurs. AMP is subsequently degraded by AMP-deaminase to IMP and ammonia. The best-known biomarkers of muscle fatigue from ATP metabolism include lactate, ammonia, and hypoxanthine. Hypoxanthine is usually analyzed in the serum or urine.

Serum lactate increases with exercise intensity in healthy and diseased subjects. Under the conditions of workload standardization, serum lactate appears to be a promising biomarker of muscle fatigue. Serum ammonia is not associated with age 77 and remains low in physical fitness, but is higher in men than in women.

Reactive oxygen species ROS remain at a low level in resting skeletal muscle but increase in response to contractile activity. ROS products lead to protein, lipid or nucleic acid oxidation accompanied by a marked decrease in the antioxidant capacity, 81 thus ultimately inducing fatigue.

Promising biomarkers to assess oxidative damage in muscle fatigue include lipid peroxidation biomarkers that is, thiobarbituric acid-reactive substances TBARS and isoprostanes , and protein oxidation biomarkers that is, protein carbonyls PCs.

Biomarkers to evaluate the antioxidant capacity include glutathione GSH , glutathione peroxidase GPX , catalase, and the total antioxidant capacity TAC. TBARS are indicators of lipid peroxidation and oxidative stress, which form during the decomposition of lipid peroxidation products that react with thiobarbituric acid and form a fluorescent red adduct.

Isoprostanes are prostaglandin-like compounds derived from the peroxidation of essential fatty acids catalyzed by ROS. PCs are mainly derived from the oxidation of albumin or other serum proteins and are regarded as markers of oxidative protein injury.

GSH is a pseudotripeptide that is present in nearly all cells and plays an important role in ROS scavenging. GPX and catalase are both enzymes that scavenge hydrogen peroxide into water and oxygen. TAC is defined as the sum of the antioxidant activities of the nonspecific pool of antioxidants.

TBARS, PC, catalase and TAC are usually determined in the serum, but TBARS are also detectable in the saliva.

Isoprostanes are usually measured in the serum, urine, or other body fluids and blood cells. GSH and GPX are present in cells and are detectable in serum and saliva.

In addition to the depletion of ATP and ROS production, exercise and fatigue also induce local or systemic inflammatory reaction. Promising biomarkers to evaluate inflammation in muscle fatigue include leukocytes, IL-6 and TNF-α.

These changes represent a nonspecific immune response induced by ischemia in a stressed tissue, while there is a lack of a real injury.

Generally, IL-6 and TNF-α levels are determined in the serum. IL-6 levels can also be determined in the saliva. With age, the change in T-cells expressing CD8 remains controversial, , whereas the change in IL-6 is age independent. Sex differences in T-cell immune responses are particularly evident in graft-versus-host disease, with a stronger effect in females, and IL-6 levels are also markedly lower in females.

There are still many potential immunological biomarkers, including C-reactive protein CRP , IL-8, IL, IL, HSP27, HSP70, plasma DNA and orosomucoid ORM. Improper exercise, long time combat, military training and some related diseases for example, cancer and stroke can cause muscle fatigue, which negatively affects athletic achievement, military combat ability and patient recovery.

At present, there are still no official or semi-official recommendations for the treatment of muscle fatigue. However, some nonspecific treatments, such as synthetic products for example, amphetamine and caffeine , natural products for example, American ginseng and rhodiola rosea and nutritional supplements for example, vitamins and minerals and creatine , have been used clinically or experimentally, and have shown some effects in various studies.

Amphetamine, ephedrine, caffeine, for example, are all synthetic products that excite the central nervous system or sympathetic nervous system and promote resistance to muscle fatigue. Almost half of the stimulant abuse in sport involves amphetamines and ephedrine, as reported by WADA World Anti-Doping Agency in The use of amphetamines, amphetamine derivatives, propanolamine and ephedrine remains illegal in competition.

However, caffeine and pseudoephedrine have been accepted at any level since Amphetamine is a phenethylamine-type stimulant and antidepressant that is highly addictive and produces euphoria and an elevated mood.

Amphetamine at low to moderate doses enhances the physical performance of humans and animals. High body temperature is one of the strongest exhaustion signals. Recently, Morozova E has reported that amphetamine may mask or delay fatigue in rats by slowing down the exercise-induced elevation in core body temperature.

Although amphetamine usage is prohibited during competitions, it may be used in some situations, such as in combat, to improve performance by delaying exhaustion. The use of caffeine as a sports-related enhancement drug is well documented.

High caffeine dose consumption enhances performance during extended periods of exercise. Other sympathomimetic stimulants, such as ephedrine, pseudoephedrine and phenylpropanolamine, are several times less potent than amphetamines in improving performance.

In addition, taltirelin, a synthetic thyrotropin-releasing hormone TRH analog, effectively improves sports activity. Benzamide derivatives, such as 1- 1, 3-benzodioxolylcarbonyl piperidine 1-BCP , significantly prolong the time of forced swimming in mice, through an unclear mechanism.

More than half of the drugs introduced worldwide are derived from or are inspired by natural products. In the past few decades, health scholars and athletic physiologists have been searching for natural products that can improve athletic ability and resist or eliminate fatigue in human beings.

Now, more and more natural products and their extracts have been revealed as potentially anti-fatigue agents. American ginseng, panax ginseng C. Meyer and radix notoginseng all belong to the araliaceae ginseng species. American ginseng is the root of panax quinquefolium, which is currently grown in Canada and eastern USA.

Panax ginseng C. ginseng is an edible and medicinal Chinese herb that is often used in Asian countries. Panax notoginseng Burk. Chen is cultivated throughout Southwest China, Burma, and Nepal. The root, a commonly used part of this plant, is called radix notoginseng or Sanchi.

All of them contain multiple active components, such as saponins, polysaccharides, flavonoids, vitamins and microelements, which are responsible for the effects in the improvement of physical fatigue in humans and animals.

For example, saponins or protein extracted from American ginseng significantly lengthens the swimming time in mice via increasing the levels of liver glycogen and muscle glycogen.

Meyer, have all been reported to have marked anti-fatigue activity in mice swimming or grasping test. Meyer, including enhancing lactate dehydrogenase LDH activity, increasing hepatic glycogen levels, retarding the accumulation of serum urea nitrogen SUN and blood lactic acid BLA , inhibiting oxidative stress and improving mitochondrial function in skeletal muscles.

Regarding panax notoginseng, a single dose has been reported to enhance aerobic capacity, endurance and mean blood pressure MAP in young adults.

Rhodiola rosea R. rosea , belonging to the family Crassulaceae and genus Rhodiola, is a commonly used plant in folk medicine in Eastern Europe and China. It is also an important resource against fatigue. The ingredients of rhodiola rosea include salidroside and rosavin.

Rosavin is the only constituent unique to R. rosea from the Rhodiola genus, and salidroside is common to most other Rhodiola species. The natural ratio of rosavins to salidrosides in R. rosea is approximately Salidroside has been identified as the main anti-fatigue ingredient in Rhodiola rosea.

rosea extract has also been found to significantly increase swimming time, hepatic superoxide dismutase content, and serum lactate dehydrogenase in mice. Garlic Allium sativum is an herb that is used mainly as a food in many countries. Garlic was given to soldiers and athletes as a tonic in ancient Rome.

Recently, the anti-fatigue effect of garlic has been reported by many researchers. Garlic-processing methods affect the anti-fatigue effects.

have examined the effect of raw garlic juice, heated garlic juice, dehydrated garlic powder and AGE on physical strength and recovery from fatigue. They have found that raw garlic and AGE prolongs the treadmill running time of mice and enhances the speed of recovery of rectal temperature after immersion in cool water.

These effects are related to the improvement of peripheral circulation, an action of anti-stress, and improvement of nutrition. have investigated the effects of garlic oil on cardiac performance and exercise tolerance in 30 patients with coronary artery disease.

After an initial treadmill stress test, the subjects were administered garlic oil for 6 weeks, and treadmill stress tests were repeated. In comparison with the initial test, garlic significantly decreased the heart rate at peak exercise and work load on the heart, thus leading to the better exercise tolerance.

Enhancing the energy metabolism effectively helps to improve exercise capacity. Chinese yam and fructus aurantii have been reported to improve muscle glycogen, liver glycogen and other indicators. However, most of them still require extensive studies to determine their anti-fatigue effects and mechanisms.

Several nutritional factors that may limit exercise performance have been identified, thus leading to the widespread use of nutritional strategies. Nutritional supplementation is regarded as legal by the International Olympic Committee IOC and, therefore, has gained popularity as a way to achieve performance enhancement.

Despite their relative paucity in the diet and the body, vitamins and minerals are key regulators of health and function, including work performance. They are not direct sources of energy but facilitate energy metabolism. Water-soluble vitamins include B vitamins thiamin, riboflavin, niacin, pyridoxine, folate, biotin, pantothenic acid, vitamin B12 and choline and vitamin C.

Fat-soluble vitamins include vitamin A, D, E, and K. Vitamin A, C and E are also antioxidants. Twelve minerals are designated essential nutrients. Magnesium, iron, zinc, copper and chromium have the potential to affect physical performance. For example, severe deprivation of folate and vitamin B12 result in anemia and decrease endurance work performance.

Iron supplementation improves progressive fatigue resistance in iron-depleted, nonanemic women. Fish oil, a dietary supplement, has been shown to have beneficial effects on performance. Fish oil is rich in omega-3 fatty acids, specifically docosahexaenoic acid DHA and eicosapentaenoic acid EPA , which have been found to improve cardiac energy efficiency, fat metabolism and immunomodulatory responses.

Creatine Cr , a nitrogen-containing compound synthesized in the body from glycine, arginine and methionine, is also found in the diet, primarily in red meat and seafood. Therefore, creatine supplementation is a potential ergogenic strategy to improve muscle endurance. Red bull contains a mixture of carbohydrates, taurine, glucuronolactone, vitamin B and caffeine, and it is a commonly used energy drink.

Several small studies have reported that carbohydrate and caffeine consumption improves aerobic and anaerobic performance as well as cognitive functions such as concentration, alertness and reaction time.

Carnitine plays an essential role in fatty acid oxidation in muscle. However, there is a lack of definite evidence regarding its beneficial role in performance as a supplement.

Protein supplements have been demonstrated to be ineffective except in rare cases in which dietary protein intake is inadequate. Individual amino acids, especially ornithine, arginine and glutamine, are also commonly used as supplements.

However, their effects on performance are not supported by documented evidence. Acute-phase protein ORM has been reported to enhance muscle endurance after vein or intraperitoneal injection in rodents, 60 but it is not convenient for daily supplementation.

In theory, the use of antioxidant vitamins and glutamine during periods of intensive training should be beneficial, but further evidence is still needed before they are recommended as supplements.

Muscle force production involves a sequence of events, extending from cortical excitation to motor unit activation to excitation—contraction coupling, and ultimately leading to muscle activation. Changes at any level in this pathway, including changes in the nervous, ion, vascular, and energy systems, impair force generation and contribute to the development of muscle fatigue.

Site-specific stimulation via non-invasive techniques provides a method to gain systemic insight into the fatigue process under physiological conditions. Although there is a lack of consensus, a sex- and age-specific distribution in muscle fatigue has been observed, in which children, older adults and males are more resistant to fatigue than adults and females.

Biomarkers of ATP metabolism, oxidative stress and inflammatory reactions are helpful for the diagnosis of muscle fatigue. Despite the lack of official or semi-official recommendations, muscle fatigue has been reported to be improved by some nonspecific treatments, including CNS-exciting drugs, natural products and nutritional supplements.

More potential mechanisms, biomarkers, targets and related drugs for muscle fatigue— for example, ORM—still need to be explored in the future. Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Gruet M, Temesi J, Rupp T, Levy P, Millet GY, Verges S. Stimulation of the motor cortex and corticospinal tract to assess human muscle fatigue. Neuroscience ; : — Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar. Norheim KB, Jonsson G, Omdal R. Biological mechanisms of chronic fatigue.

Rheumatology Oxford ; 50 : — Article CAS Google Scholar. Silverman MN, Heim CM, Nater UM, Marques AH, Sternberg EM. Neuroendocrine and immune contributors to fatigue. PM R ; 2 : — Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar.

Kroenke K, Wood DR, Mangelsdorff AD, Meier NJ, Powell JB. Chronic fatigue in primary care. Prevalence, patient characteristics, and outcome. JAMA ; : — Gandevia SC. Spinal and supraspinal factors in human muscle fatigue. Physiol Rev ; 81 : — Bigland-Ritchie B, Jones DA, Hosking GP, Edwards RH.

Central and peripheral fatigue in sustained maximum voluntary contractions of human quadriceps muscle. Clin Sci Mol Med ; 54 : — CAS PubMed Google Scholar.

Kent-Braun JA, Fitts RH, Christie A. Skeletal muscle fatigue. Compr Physiol ; 2 : — PubMed Google Scholar. Swart J, Lamberts RP, Lambert MI, Lambert EV, Woolrich RW, Johnston S et al.

Exercising with reserve: exercise regulation by perceived exertion in relation to duration of exercise and knowledge of endpoint. Br J Sports Med ; 43 : — Klass M, Roelands B, Lévénez M, Fontenelle V, Pattyn N, Meeusen R et al. Effects of noradrenaline and dopamine on supraspinal fatigue in well-trained men.

Med Sci Sports Exerc ; 44 : — Roelands B, Goekint M, Heyman E, Piacentini MF, Watson P, Hasegawa H et al. Acute norepinephrine reuptake inhibition decreases performance in normal and high ambient temperature.

J Appl Physiol ; : — Piacentini MF, Meeusen R, Buyse L, De SG, Kempenaers F, Van NJ et al. No effect of a noradrenergic reuptake inhibitor on performance in trained cyclists.

Med Sci Sports Exerc ; 34 : — Enoka RM, Fuglevand AJ. Motor unit physiology: some unresolved issues. Muscle nerve ; 24 : 4— Heckman CJ, Enoka RM.

Motor unit. Taylor JL, Amann M, Duchateau J, Meeusen R, Rice CL. Neural contributions to muscle fatigue: from the brain to the muscle and back again.

Med Sci Sports Exerc ; 48 : — Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar. Darques JL, Decherchi P, Jammes Y. Mechanisms of fatigue-induced activation of group IV muscle afferents: the roles played by lactic acid and inflammatory mediators. Neurosci Lett ; : — Darques JL, Jammes Y.

Fatigue-induced changes in group IV muscle afferent activity: differences between high- and low-frequency electrically induced fatigues. Brain Res ; : — Vie B, Gomez N, Brerro-Saby C, Weber JP, Jammes Y. Changes in stationary upright standing and proprioceptive reflex control of foot muscles after fatiguing static foot inversion.

J Biomech ; 46 : — Article PubMed Google Scholar. Brerro-Saby C, Delliaux S, Steinberg JG, Jammes Y. Fatigue-induced changes in tonic vibration response TVR in humans: relationships between electromyographic and biochemical events.

Muscle Nerve ; 38 : — Fichna J, Poole DP, Veldhuis N, MacEachern SJ, Saur D, Zakrzewski PK et al. Transient receptor potential vanilloid 4 inhibits mouse colonic motility by activating NO-dependent enteric neurotransmission.

J Mol Med Berl ; 93 : — MacIntosh BR, Holash RJ, Renaud JM. Skeletal muscle fatigue—regulation of excitation-contraction coupling to avoid metabolic catastrophe. J Cell Sci ; : — Allen DG, Lamb GD, Westerblad H.

Impaired calcium release during fatigue. Wright JR, McCloskey DI, Fitzpatrick RC. Effect of muscle perfusion on fatigue andsystemic arterial pressure i human subjects.

J Appl Physiol ; 86 : — Degens H, Salmons S, Jarvis JC. Intramuscular pressure, force and blood flow in rabbit tibialis anterior muscles during single and repetitive contractions.

Eur J Appl Physiol Occup Physiol ; 78 : 13— Pitcher JB, Miles TS. Influence of muscle blood flow on fatigue during intermittent human hand-grip exercise and recovery. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol ; 24 : — Tachi M, Kouzaki M, Kanehisa H, Fukunaga T.

The influence of circulatory difference on muscle oxygenation and fatigue during intermittent static dorsiflexion. Eur J Appl Physiol ; 91 : — Degens H, Sanchez Horneros JM, Hopman MT.

Acute hypoxia limits endurance but does not affect muscle contractile properties. Muscle Nerve ; 33 : — Cole MA, Brown MD.

Response of the human triceps surae muscle to electrical stimulation during varying levels of blood flow restriction. Eur J Appl Physiol ; 82 : 39— Lanza IR, Wigmore DM, Befroy DE, Kent-Braun JA.

In vivo ATP production during free-flow and ischaemic muscle contractions in humans. J Physiol ; : — Wigmore DM, Propert K, Kentbraun JA.

Blood flow does not limit skeletal muscle force production during incremental isometric contractions. Eur J Appl Physiol ; 96 : — Amann M, Romer LM, Subudhi AW, Pegelow DF, Dempsey JA. Severity of arterial hypoxaemia affects the relative contributions of peripheral muscle fatigue to exercise performance in healthy humans.

Amann M, Eldridge MW, Lovering AT, Stickland MK, Pegelow DF, Dempsey JA. Arterial oxygenation influences central motor output and exercise performance via effects on peripheral locomotor muscle fatigue in humans.

Grassi B, Rossiter HB, Hogan MC, Howlett RA, Harris JE, Goodwin ML et al. Faster O 2 uptake kinetics in canine skeletal muscle in situ after acute creatine kinase inhibition.

Kent JA, Ortenblad N, Hogan MC, Poole DC, Musch TI. No Muscle Is an Island: Integrative Perspectives on Muscle Fatigue. Homsher E. Muscle enthalpy production and its relationship to actomyosin ATPase. Annu Rev Physiol ; 49 : — Ashar FN, Moes A, Moore AZ, Grove ML, Chaves PHM, Coresh J et al.

Association of mitochondrial DNA levels with frailty and all-cause mortality. Nielsen J, Ortenblad N. Physiological aspects of the subcellular localization of glycogen in skeletal muscle. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab ; 38 : 91— Ortenblad N, Westerblad H, Nielsen J.

Muscle glycogen stores and fatigue. Bergstrom J, Hermansen L, Hultman E, Saltin B. Diet, muscle glycogen and physical performance. Acta Physiol Scand ; 71 : — Hermansen L, Hultman E, Saltin B. Muscle glycogen during prolonged severe exercise. Hargreaves M, McConell G, Proietto J. Influence of muscle glycogen on glycogenolysis and glucose uptake during exercise in humans.

J Appl Physiol ; 78 : — Sahlin K, Tonkonogi M, Soderlund K. Energy supply and muscle fatigue in humans. Acta Physiol Scand ; : — Chin ER, Allen DG. J Physiol ; : 17— Nielsen J, Schroder HD, Rix CG, Ortenblad N.

Distinct effects of subcellular glycogen localization on tetanic relaxation time and endurance in mechanically skinned rat skeletal muscle fibres. Ortenblad N, Nielsen J, Saltin B, Holmberg HC. Article PubMed CAS Google Scholar. Metzger JM, Moss RL. Effects of tension and stiffness due to reduced pH in mammalian fast- and slow-twitch skinned skeletal muscle fibres.

Pate E, Bhimani M, Franks-Skiba K, Cooke R. Reduced effect of pH on skinned rabbit psoas muscle mechanics at high temperatures: implications for fatigue. Stackhouse SK, Reisman DS, Binder-Macleod SA. Challenging the role of pH in skeletal muscle fatigue.

Call or Chat now! Simply defined, yet physiologically quite complex, fatigue refers to the inability to continue exercise at a given intensity. This article provides a brief overview of the various types of fatigue and examines the role gender plays in fatigue.

Also covered are training recommendations for combatting fatigue. Fatigue is a process that develops over time, and is largely dependent on the duration and intensity of exercise. The three dominant exercise scenarios leading to fatigue are short-term intense exercise, repeated-sprint exercise and extended submaximal exercise Girard et al.

Following is an overview of the key metabolic and system events that contribute to fatigue under these circumstances.

During activities such as sprinting and high-intensity resistance exercise, continued muscle contraction is dependent on the formation of adenosine triphosphate ATP for the demanding energy needs. Under these exercise conditions, creatine phosphate CrP , which resynthesizes ATP, and glucose breakdown called glycolysis are primarily responsible for maintaining ATP levels.

Skeletal muscle concentrations of CrP are quite limited; in fact, resting CrP stores can be depleted within approximately 10 seconds Powers and Howley, Historically, it was thought that lactate was principally a dead-end waste product that caused muscle soreness and fatigue.

Conversely, proton accumulation contributes to a decreased cellular pH acidosis , which interferes with normal skeletal muscle contraction through a number of mechanisms:.

Activity of the enzyme myosin ATPase is impaired. This enzyme is responsible for the breakdown of ATP in skeletal muscle and the release of energy to support muscle contraction. Other enzymes involved in the cellular regulation of sodium and potassium during muscle contraction become impaired.

Training can partially mitigate these negative events. Check out the box below for advice on how to combat fatigue during short-term, intense exercise.

Fatigue-fighting Training Tip 1. Improvements to the buffering-capacity system of the cellular environment will permit better tolerance of increased acidosis and, as a result, delay fatigue when performing short-term intense exercise.

Interestingly, past research has shown that eliciting acidosis i. Repeated-sprint exercise RSE is a fitness attribute required for success in many team and racquet sports Girard et al. RSE is characterized by numerous short-duration sprints generally lasting less than 10 to 15 seconds interspersed with brief and incomplete recovery typically less than 60 seconds.

Many of the sources of fatigue for RSE are similar to those encountered in short-term intense exercise, including proton accumulation and a finite CrP supply.

Exercise conditions, such as RSE, that extensively tax the phosphagen system ultimately lead to depletion in the primary fuel i. Therefore, recovery from CrP degradation becomes critical if the later bouts of RSE are to be maintained at a high performance.

Fatigue-fighting training tip 2 below highlights how combatting fatigue during RSE can be augmented. A neural fatigue also exists during RSE. Each single motor nerve which are under your voluntary control activates a group of muscle fibers ranging from a few to several hundred or more , and is collectively referred to as a motor unit.

Fatigue-fighting Training T ip 2. Research suggests that individuals with higher maximal oxygen uptake VO 2 max values have a greater capacity to resist fatigue during RSE.

The most likely reason is that those with high VO 2 max levels also more rapidly resynthesize CrP stores between brief bouts of intense exercise. Traditionally, increasing cardiorespiratory fitness has been accomplished through the performance of moderate-intensity continuous exercise training.

More contemporary research has shown interval training to also be a great option for quickly improving VO 2 max. This workout should be performed one to two days per week Gormley et al.

During prolonged exercises such as cycling, cross-country skiing and distance running, muscle contraction is also dependent on the ability of metabolic pathways breakdown of a fuel to release energy to continuously regenerate ATP.

Mitochondrial respiration aerobic metabolism in the mitochondrion of the cell becomes the primary supplier of ATP. Many fuels, or substrates from fat, carbohydrates and proteins, are available for mitochondrial respiration; however, the two most important relative to fatigue are blood glucose and muscle glycogen.

Fats in the form of triglycerides are also readily available for ATP production, but their breakdown is much slower than glucose and glycogen.

Decreased levels of blood glucose and low levels of muscle glycogen have been more associated with the onset of fatigue in sustained exercise events.

In terms of optimizing exercise performance, a limiting factor to glycolysis, and maintenance of this maximal rate of ATP regeneration, is the substrate availability of muscle glycogen.

Research has reported that muscle glycogen stores become depleted after approximately two hours of maximal usage Fitts, Prolonged aerobic and resistance exercise eccentric muscle contractions, in particular can lead to substantial disturbances in the skeletal muscle environment, including damage to the sarcolemma, contractile proteins and connective tissue Powers and Howley, These disturbances can impair the ability to transport blood glucose into the skeletal muscle cell, which in turn leads to a decreased capacity to replenish glycogen stores.

As a result, fatigue may continue for numerous days after the workout. Some exercise activities, such as hiking, cross-training events and endurance races, may take place in hot, humid environments that can lead to considerable heat stress and possibly dehydration.

Repeated muscle contractions during prolonged exercise leads to continuous, metabolically generated heat, which can dramatically increase core body temperature a condition known as hyperthermia.

Research suggests that rising core temperatures may cause fatigue in both the contracting muscles and central nervous system Powers and Howley, Failure to maintain fluid balance throughout prolonged sports events and exercise sessions may eventually result in decreased availability of blood flow to both exercising muscles and the skin for dissipation of heat.

The importance of fluid intake for total-body health, sport performance and reduced fatigue cannot be overemphasized. The third training tip, below, explains how to improve exercise tolerance during exposure to hot and humid environments. Fatigue-fighting Training T ip 3. Chronic adaptations associated with heat acclimation include increased plasma volume, earlier onset of sweating during exercise combined with enhanced sweat rate, and decreased electrolyte loss.

The stimulus required for eliciting these favorable adaptations is training in hot environments. The scientific literature has clearly shown, however, that the intensity of these training sessions need not be high.

For example, it was recently reported Lorenzo et al.

Physical exercise psrformance the Fayigue of the Anv environment. During exercise the contracting muscles generate force or power and heat. So physical exercise Meal prepping ideas performancd fact a form of Fatigue and exercise performance Diabetes and sleep disorders. This generated energy will deplete the energy stocks within the body. During exercise, metabolites and heat are generated, which affect the steady state of the internal environment. Depending on the form of exercise, sooner or later sensations of fatigue and exhaustion will occur. The physiological role of these sensations is protection of the exercising subject from the deleterious effects of exercise. Fatigue and exercise performance

Author: Akit

1 thoughts on “Fatigue and exercise performance

Leave a comment

Yours email will be published. Important fields a marked *

Design by ThemesDNA.com